Studies on ABA/VB
o
1. Maximizing the potential for infants at-risk for autism spectrum disorder through a parent-mediated verbal behavior intervention
Summary of the study
Amy Tanner & Katerina Dounavi
The human brain undergoes the most transformative development period in all post-natal life from birth to 36 months, going from simple connections to complex connections responsible for social behavior, communication and cognition (Courchesne & Pierce, 2005). Over the past five years, an increase in pre-diagnostic intervention for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), also known as pre-emptive intervention (prior to 18 months of age), has been noticed. During this time, neuroplasticity is at its peak, allowing for more rapid changes in development and ultimately more effective intervention (Bradshaw, Steiner, Gengoux, & Koegal, 2015). It is essential to begin intervention while these connections are still being formed rather than trying to change maladaptive neural connections once they are established (Pierce et al., 2016). In fact, findings from previous studies indicated that children diagnosed with ASD who began behavioral intervention prior to two years of age were 60% more likely to make significant gains in their first year of intervention compared to those who began after 30 months of age (MacDonald et al., 2014).
Research studying the efficacy of pre-emptive interventions utilized early screening tools and a parent-mediated service delivery model that aims to improve key parental skills that can mitigate ASD symptoms. Target behaviors selected for increase included among others: joint attention, responsivity to parent, social smiling and social orienting along with receptive and expressive language, while the severity of prodromal autism symptoms have been targeted for a decrease.
The present study assessed the effectiveness of a 12-week parent-mediated pre-diagnostic intervention program aiming to reduce autism symptoms and increase appropriate social communication and play behaviors in high risk infants.
Method
The research design was a multiple baseline design across five parent-child dyads. The intervention consisted in twelve 1‑hour coaching sessions delivered by a Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA®), followed by a 1 and 3‑month follow-up session.
Child participants were between 12 and 16 months of age and comprised three girls and two boys, while the primary parent participant of each dyad was the child’s biological mother. Three of the five children were an “only-child”, while two were “high-risk siblings” (they had an older sibling with a confirmed ASD diagnosis).
Intervention
Α BCBA® conducted all coaching sessions following a Behavior Skills Training (BST) protocol incorporating instruction, modelling, rehearsal and feedback into each session. At the end of each session, specific goals were selected for parents to practice during the upcoming week. Topics were framed using Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior (Skinner, 1957) with target behaviors including verbal operants such as mands, tacts and echoics, as well as imitation and play skills.
Measurement
For data collection, three primary and four secondary variables were measured. Primary variables included parental and infant behaviors. Parental target behaviors were: (1) the number of learning opportunities the parent presented by providing the infant with an opportunity i) to respond to a discriminative stimulus, ii) to mand for an item or activity, iii) to imitate a motor or vocal response, and iv) to respond to a clear instruction. Infant target behaviors included: (1) the frequency of eye contact directed towards the parent and (2) the frequency of responding to the learning opportunity presented by the parent.
Parental fidelity of implementation was assessed, as well as social validity.
Results
The outcomes with regard to eye-contact demonstrated the most significant increase out of all three infant target behaviors, with a mean increase of 40% across all five infants and results maintaining at the 3‑month follow-up.
Infants’ responsivity to learning opportunities also showed a significant increase of 41% on average compared to baseline across all infants. The effect size estimators for children responsivity, children eye contact and parent behavior indicated a large or very large improvement. Additionally, all five infants showed a decrease in autism symptoms and an increase in appropriate acquisition behaviors within the 12-week intervention period. However, four of the five infants still received a diagnosis of ASD between 18 and 22 months of age.
Social validity assessments were also conducted during the 3‑month follow-up and showed that the intervention was widely acceptable by parents, with 96% of questions rating the intervention as either positive or very positive.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to assess the efficacy and acceptability of a pre-emptive behavioral intervention for infants at risk of ASD. The five infants showed approximately a 10-month gain of skills that would not be accounted for by maturation alone. The goal of pre-diagnostic intervention is not to eliminate the future diagnosis of ASD but rather to maximize the potential of infants who are showing early signs of ASD and alter their developmental trajectory. The study is unique in that it provides directions on how to deliver such an intervention building capacity among behavior analysts and allowing more families to access pre-diagnostic services, therefore improving the well-being of individuals. Behavior analysts are trained to identify socially significant target behaviors and should begin treatment regardless of the presence or absence of an ASD diagnosis and as soon as the first concerns around the child’s development are expressed.
The results of the present study add to the massive literature that has examined the effect of behavioral interventions on a child’s outcomes showing that Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) is the scientific basis of the treatment of choice for ASD. Additionally, the present study provides support to the existing literature in the use of low-intensity parent-mediated intervention, which can be implemented by any parent or entry-level professional, under the guidance of a qualified professional (i.e., BCBA®).
References
- Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Brain overgrowth in autism during a critical time in development: Implications for frontal pyramidal neuron and interneuron development and connectivity. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 153–170.
- Bradshaw, J., Steiner, A. M., Gengoux, G., & Koegal, L. (2015). Feasibility and effectiveness of very early intervention for infants at-risk for autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Journal of Autism and Developmental, 45(3), 778.
- Pierce, K., Courchesne, E., & Bacon, E. (2016). To screen or not to screen universally for autism is not the question: Why the task force got it wrong. The Journal of Pediatrics, 176, 182–194.
- MacDonald, R., Parry-Cruwys, D., Dupere, S., & Adhern, W. (2014). Assessing progress and outcome of early intensive behavioral intervention for toddlers with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 3632–3644.
- Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Zusammenfassung von Sophia Patrogiannaki and Katerina Dounavi, http://magiko-sympan.gr/blog/maximizing-the-potential-for-infants-at-risk-for-autism-spectrum-disorder-through-a-parent-mediated-verbal-behavior-intervention/
Original article here.
o
2. ABA- based interventions for individuals with Down’s syndrome
Summary of the study
Katerina Dounavi
Clinical director of Melody Learning Center, lecturer at school of education, Queen’s University Belfast.
More than forty years of research have well documented that interventions based on the science of Applied Behaviour Analysis (ABA) are the best available choice for children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD). This can be claimed since ABA-based interventions have proven to effectively develop social and communication skills, reduce inappropriate behaviours (e.g., aggressive behaviours, stereotyped behaviours), and facilitate meaningful inclusion. (e.g., New Zealand Guidelines Group, 2008; Surgeon General, 1999).
The fact that evidence-based interventions driven from ABA have shown to be effective for children with ASD has often lead to the misconception that ABA is synonymous to a “therapy for autism” and that it can only be used to design interventions for children with ASD. As some authors have already pointed out (e.g., Dillenburger & Keenan, 2009), this is a myth. ABA is the science that focuses on socially significant human behaviour (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007), thus it can and should be the basis for the design of an effective intervention for improving any aspect of human behaviour (e.g., increasing communication, social, or academic skills and decreasing inappropriate behaviours such as aggressive or self-injurious behaviours) for any population (e.g., adults with aphasia, children with ASD, adults learning a second language, children with Down’s syndrome, etc.) In the last decades, and as the scientific evidence for the effectiveness of ABA-based interventions for individuals with ASD has dramatically increased, there is a parallel increase of research activity in relation to the effectiveness of ABA-based interventions for other populations. This research activity has included children with Down’s syndrome, children with learning disorders, individuals with eating disorders, individuals exhibiting gambling behaviours, adults with depression, post-stroke aphasia patients, and numerous other areas (e.g. respectively, Athens, Vollmer, Sloman, & St Peter Pipkin, 2008; Sidman & Kirk, 1974; Seiverling, Williams, Sturmey, & Hart, 2012; Nastally, Dixon, & Jackson, 2010; Kanter, Callaghan, Landes, Busch, & Brown, 2004; Baker, LeBlanc, & Raetz, 2008).
More in detail, research on the effectiveness of specific procedures derived from the science of ABA for teaching different skills to children with Down’s syndrome have yelled consistently positive results. In 1973, Dalton, Rubino, and Hislop showed how the implementation of a token economy system could effectively produce improvements in the performance of 13 children with Down’s syndrome with ages ranging from 6 to 14 years old. In 1978, Farb and Throne put in place a training program with the aim to improve the generalized mnemonic performance (i.e., memory) of a 7 years old girl with Down’s syndrome. In 1989, Drash, Raver, Murrin, and Tudor compared three procedures aiming to increase the early vocal responses of 25 children with Down’s syndrome and concluded that light- dimming and screening combined with positive reinforcement produced the most significant increases. In a study published in 1993 (Lalli, Browder, Mace, & Brown) including a 10 years old boy with Down’s syndrome, the authors proved the effectiveness of a behaviour-analytic procedure in decreasing students’ problem behaviour and concurrently increasing their verbal skills during natural classroom activities taking place in a public school. In a study conducted with five children among whom two boys with Down’s syndrome, McComas, Thompson, and Johnson (2003) showed how functional analysis methodology, one of the most rigorously tested methodologies in behaviour analysis, can prove effective in identifying the underlying causes of problem behaviors and thus assist in putting effective interventions in place. For a detailed review on the use of analysis, assessment, and interventions derived from ABA to treat challenging behaviours shown by individuals with Down’s syndrome, the reader is encouraged to visit Feeley and Jones’ (2006) study. Finally, some other authors (Athens, Vollmer, Sloman, & St Peter Pipkin, 2008) demonstrated how to reduce vocal stereotypies of an 11-year old boy with Down’s syndrome and autism. The list of studies testing the effectiveness of behaviour-analytic procedures with children, teenagers, and adults with Down’s syndrome is not infinite yet. But there are already robust research results indicating the beneficial effects that can be gathered from using ABA-based interventions for this population. The above mentioned studies constitute only a small number out of the entire existing literature; thus, researchers, practitioners, parents, and policy-makers are encouraged not to rely only on these sources. A detailed investigation should be conducted in order to identify all the available evidence up to this moment in relation to the use of ABA with individuals with Down’s syndrome.
References
- Athens, E. S., Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & ST Peter Pipkin, C. (2008). An analysis of vocal stereotypy and therapist fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 291 297.
- Baker, J. C., LeBlanc, L. A., & Raetz, P. G. (2008). A behavioral conceptualization of aphasia. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 147- 158.
- Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. 2nd edition. Pearson, Merrill: Prentice Hall.
- Dalton, A. J., Rubino, K. A., & Hislop, M. W. (1973). Some effects of token rewards on school achievement of children with Down s syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 251–259.
- Drash, P.W., Raver, S.A., Murrin, M.R., & Tudor, R.M. (1989). Three procedures for increasing vocal response to therapist prompt in infants and children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 94, 64–73.
- Dillenburger, K. & Keenan, M. (2009). None of the As in ABA stands for autism: Dispelling the myths. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability. 34, 193–195.
- Farb, J. & Throne, J. M. (1978). Improving the generalized mnemonic performance of a Down s syndrome child. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 413–419.
- Feeley, K. M. & Jones, E. A. (2006). Addressing challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome: The use of applied behaviour analysis for assessment and intervention. Down Syndrome Research and Practice 1, 64–77.
- Kanter, J. W., Callaghan, G. M., Landes, S. J., Busch, A. M., & Brown, K. R. (2004). Behavior Analytic Conceptualization and Treatment of Depression: Traditional models and recent advances. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5, 255–274.
- Lalli, J. S., Browder, D. M., Mace, F. C., & Brown, D. K. (1993). Teacher use of descriptive analysis data to implement interventions to decrease students problem behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 227–238.
- Nastally, B. L., Dixon, M. R., & Jackson, J. W. (2010). Manipulating slot machine preference in problem gamblers through contextual control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 125–129.
- New Zealand Guidelines Group. The effectiveness of applied behaviour analysis interventions for people with autism spectrum disorder. Systematic Review. Wellington; 2008
- Sidman, M. & Kirk, B. (1974). Letter Reversals in Naming, Writing, and Matching to Sample. Child Development, 45, 616–625.
- Surgeon General. (1999). Mental health: A report of the Surgeon General. U.S. Public Health Service. Retrieved on 21/11/2012 from http://profiles.nlm.nih.gov/ps/retrieve/ResourceMetadata/NNBBJC. Seiverling, L., Williams, K., Sturmey, P. & Hart, S. (2012). Effects of behavioral skills training on parental treatment of children s food selectivity. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 197–203.
Great thanks to Katerina Dounavi for writing this report and giving permission to post it on the MLC website.
Thanks to Xenia Weinmann for translating the report from English to German.
Thanks a lot for allowing this study to be summarized, translated and published to: Kathy Hill, business manager of JABA
For the summary and the translation a heartfelt thank you to Anne Burzinski.
o
3. An experimental analysis of facilitated communication (FC)
Summary of the study
Barbara B. Montee, Raymond G Miltenberger, David Wittrock (North Dakota State Universität)
Facilitated communication is a method of physical assistance to help an intellectually impaired person to communicate. The facilitator supports the hand of the client, who uses his index finger to point to letters on a letter board or to type on en electronic keyboard. According to Biklen and others, facilitated communication enables individuals with autism and those with other developmental disabilities to communicate. They were suggesting that they are not intellectually impaired. Other researchers, such as Wheeler, Jacobson, Paglieri, Schwartz and others, argued hat facilitator control of the typing was the most plausible explanation for the messages typed during facilitated communication sessions. To prove this there were proceeded a lot of experiments.
Experiments in which clients who had demonstrated unexpected literacy were shown pictures. They were asked to type the names of the pictures using facilitated communication support this assumption. These experiments demonstrate that the name of the picture was typed in correctly only when the facilitator was shown the same picture. When the facilitator was shown a different picture or no picture at all, the client-facilitator pair never typed in the correct name. Biklen in return criticized the results by supporting that too many experimental arrangements can make clients anxious, that testing destroys trust between the pair, that neither the facilitator nor the pair had received enough training and that the participants had aphasia.
Adressing Biklen’s commentaries, in the present study Montee et al analysed 7 client-facilitator pairs that had been using facilitated communication for 6 to 18 months. The clients were adults diagnosed with moderate or severe mental retardation with secondary diagnoses such as cerebral palsy, epilepsy, autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or pervasive developmental disorder. They used two evaluation formats: describing activities and naming pictures.
They addressed the criticism raised by Biklen former in the following ways: 1) They used client-facilitator pairs that already had a lot of experience with facilitated communication. 2) A baseline condition was always conducted to establish successful communication and to make sure there are no word-finding problems. 3) The clients did not have to exactly name an object. It was also okay to describe it. Als richtige Antwort wurde auch eine richtige Umschreibung gezählt. 4) Anxiety and escape behaviours were measured in every experimental session. If such behaviour was measured, the experimental data were not used. 5) All sessions were conducted in their usual locations, at the usual time and with the usual facilitator in order to reduce the potential for anxiety or other negative reactions to the experiment. 6) Any time that the facilitator was not comfortable for any reason, the experimental trial was terminated.
The basic experimental manipulation was the control of the facilitator’s access to information about an activity or a picture. There were three experimental conditions: known (the facilitator had knowledge of the activity or the picture), unknown (the facilitator did not have knowledge of the activity or picture) and false information (the facilitator was given false information about the activity of picture).
In the activity format the client engaged in a familiar activity for about 5 minutes. Examples of activities included drinking coffee, looking at a magazine, eating soda crackers, playing cards and putting together a puzzle. Immediately after the activity the researcher either told the facilitator what the activity was (known), provided no information (unknown) or gave the facilitator information on an activity that did not take place (false). In the picture format the client and facilitator were shown the same picture (known), only the client was presented a picture (unknown) or the pictures presented to the client and facilitator were different. After the experimental trials the facilitators were asked to fill out a questionnaire that assessed the degree to which they believed that they influenced the facilitated communication during the experimental sessions.
The results show no great difference between the two evaluation formats naming a picture and describing an activity. The percentage of correct responses was high for all clients in the known condition and was at or near zero in the unknown and false condition. The results for the facilitator questionnaire show that the facilitators estimated that the clients performed better when the facilitator had knowledge of the correct answer, but they also estimated that the clients answered correctly more often than not in false and unknown conditions. They were convinced that the clients largely controlled the communication during sessions.
Due to these results three main conclusions were drawn from the study. First, consistent with prior research, facilitated communication did not lead to communication that came from the client. Second, the facilitators controlled the communication even though all of them believed that the client was authoring the messages. Regarding the issue of facilitator control it is noteworthy that there was a 23% refusal rate to answer in the unknown condition compared to refusal rates of 3% and 7% in the known and false condition. Also, when the facilitator did not have knowledge of the picture or activity, it took longer for most pairs to respond than in the other two conditions. A third conclusion is that anxiety and avoidance behaviours can not be counted as a reason for the failure to find facilitated communication. Only three of 320 trials were terminated due to such behaviour. Finally the fourth conclusion is that there was no difference in responses to the activity and picture scenarios. Therefore word-finding difficulties are no reason to question the validity of the results.
These results and the results of previous studies come to the conclusion that facilitated communication is not a valid means of improving communication. Therefore the facilitated communication should not be used any more. Those who continue using facilitated communication should consider the following implications: First, every message produced through facilitated communication should be verified through other means such as verbalizations or sign language. Second, the client and legal guardian should be informed of the risk of the facilitator’s influence when using facilitated communication. Third, it is important that other existing ways to communicate are not ignored in favor of facilitated communication.
Finally, facilitated communication for the first time led to the belief that persons with disabilities have more normal intelligence than thought. They were treated with more dignity and respect by their caregivers and family. Agencies must ensure that staff members continue to treat individuals with dignity and respect in the absence of facilitated communication.
For reading the Comprehensive version and for More information, please download JABA’S study.(JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS, 1995,282189–200, NUMBER2 (SUMMER195))
Thanks a lot for allowing this study to be summarized, translated and published to: Kathy Hill, business manager of JABA
For the summary and the translation a heartfelt thank you to Anne Burzinski.
o
4. ABA for older children — Supporting evidence
Summary of the study
By Dr. Katerina Dounavi, BCBA‑D
Applied Behaviour Analysis (ABA) is the applied branch of the science called Behaviour Analysis. The term “Applied” refers to the application of the findings of the scientific study of behaviour to socially relevant targets. ABA has proven effective with different populations and in different areas, such as in treating individuals with autism and adults with aphasia, in the area of business organization, in designing effective interventions for children with learning disabilities, in treating phobias, etc.
One of the principal areas where ABA has demonstrated its effectiveness is in teaching skills to and treating behavioural problems of children with autism. There is an extensive scientific literature based on research conducted during the last 40 years approximately, which supports ABA as the ideal intervention for individuals with autism (e.g., Kuppens & Onghena, 2011; Eldevik, Hastings, Hughes, Jahr, Eikeseth, & Cross, 2009; McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993; Reichow & Wolery, 2009), demonstrates its superiority over eclectic treatments (Dillenburger, 2011a; Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, & Stanislaw, 2005; Osborne & Reed, 2008; Zachor, Ben-Itschak, Rabinovich, & Lahat, 2007) and shows that parental stress declines after the intervention (Dillenburger, Keenan, Gallagher, & McElhinney, 2004).
Most of this research is conducted with pre-school and school-age children but in the last years there is a growing number of studies focusing on treatment of adolescents, youths and adults with autism. The results are equally interesting for proving that ABA can be effective independently of the individual’s age. One misconception that has up to date guided government policies related to funding but fortunately not clinical practice and scientific research is that ABA is effective only with children of pre-school age. There is now enough supporting evidence suggesting that if an intervention based on ABA starts before the age of 4 years, the outcomes are higher than starting later. This conclusion has led to the false belief that ABA is not effective in children of school age, adolescents, youths and adults. There is no scientific evidence up to date clncluding that intervention should be stopped at a specific age due to its lack of effectivity. Instead there is growing research evidence for the opposite conclusion, positive and valuable gains. For example, McEachin, Smith and Lovaas (1993) indicate that it took six years for one of the best-outcome children to reach typical functioning.
The misconception that ABA is not effective after the age of 7 can be very prejudicial to thousands of children, adolescents, youths and adults, as decisions related to their treatment directly influence their quality of life and the quality of life of their broader social network. Limiting a beneficial intervention due to a misconception that is not evidence-based would lead to a child acquiring fewer skills than possible, maintaining non-desired and non-adaptive behaviours in his repertoire due to the lack of specialized treatment, showing decreased independent skills, reducing his possibilities of integration, etc.
Given the above, there is a clear need for conducting more research studies showing how methods based on the science of ABA can be effective with older children, adolescents, youths and adults. In the following paragraphs, we will do a very brief revision of the existing body of literature supporting the effectiveness of ABA with children older than 7 years old, adolescents, youths and adults. Also, we will provide some data on brain development as it has been described in studies that use neuro-imaging, in order to give a clear picture of the learning possibilities of an individual independently of his age.
Several review articles and meta-analyses have been published summarizing the large body of literature (thousands of studies) supporting ABA-based intervention as the most effective one for individuals with autism (e.g., Eikeseth, 2009; Howard, Sparkman, Choen, Green, & Stanislaw, 2005; Koegel, Koegel, Harrower, & Carter, 1999; Krantz & McClannahan, 1993; Lovaas, 1987). These studies describe effective procedures developed across a wide range of skills and problem behaviours, such as language and communication (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985; Durand, & Carr, 1992; Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998), daily living skills (e.g., Horner & Keilitz, 1975), academic skills and school integration (e.g., Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992; Daly & Martens, 1994; McComas, Wacker, & Cooper, 1996), reduction of stereotypical behaviour (e.g., Dounavi, 2011) and other.
In these studies, participants are individuals with autism of all ages, from pre-school children to adults, so demonstrating the effectiveness of the ABA-based procedures independently of the individual’s age. One of the most interesting studies offering support to ABA-based intervention at a later age, is the one conducted by Harris and Handleman (2000) in which the authors clearly state that great benefits were observed following ABA intervention with older children as well. Additionally, there is a large number of small sample sized studies, which have demonstrated the effectiveness of ABA to teach specific skills in different areas and reduce problem behaviours of various types to adolescents, youths and adults with autism. Here, we will only mention some of them. Haring, Roger, Lee, Breen and Gaylord-Ross (1992) demonstrated the effectiveness of a social network intervention for youths with moderate and severe disabilities, including autism, by measuring the frequency, number and appropriateness of social interactions. There has been used a multiple baseline design and showed that the intervention was successful in increasing the quantity and quality of interactions and promoting friendships. Other studies have also demonstrated effective ABA-based procedures for youth population (e.g., Haring, Roger, Lee, Breen, & Gaylord-Ross, 1984; McMorrow & Foxx, 1986; Gena, Krantz, McClannahan, & Poulson, 1996; McGee, Krantz, Mason, & McClannahan, 1983).
School age children between the age of 7 and before the start of adolescence have also been significant in proving the importance of following an evidence-based intervention, ABA. For example, Taylor & Levin (1998) demonstrated the effectiveness of a prompting technique for a 9‑year-old student with autism to make verbal initiations about his play activities. Blew, Schwartz and Luce (1985) described how older children with autism were taught community skills, such as crossing the street, making purchases, and checking out books from the library, and other.
During these years, most of the children that have already followed an ABA-based intervention during preschool age are now in need of an ABA-intervention that will guide their integration in mainstream schools, design effective individualized educational programmes for social interactions with peers and teach academic skills in an effective way. Frequently, adolescents are in need of similar support provided through ABA-based services. There are plenty of examples of research studies that focus on the acquisition of these skills, such as how to train shadow teachers to support the integration of children with autism in the mainstream classroom (Monahan & Bryer, 2004).
Studies focusing on adolescents have been numerous. They have demonstrated significant effects of ABA-based interventions to improve skills acquisition and reduction of problem behaviour (e.g., Miller & Neuringer, 2000). For example, Delano (2007) showed how to improve language performance of adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. Palmen, Didden and Arts (2008) showed how to improve question asking in high-functioning adolescents with autism.
The research about adults is also extensive and focuses on different areas, such as sign language (Schepis, Reid, Fitzgerald, Faw, VanDenPol, & Welty, 1982) independent life skills [Haring, Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-Conway, 1987), job skills for laboural integration [Smith & Coleman, 1986], reduction of aggressive behaviour (Hagopian & Adelinis, 2001; Thompson & Iwata, 2001; Lalli, Mace, Wohn, & Livezey, 1995) and other.
Regarding brain development, one of the arguments often used to support the non-evidence based view that funding should be stopped at a certain age is that after early childhood the human brain is not flexible and, therefore, further development is negligible. Here, we briefly summarize the scientific conclusions that broadly show that ABA-based intervention brings very significant gains to individuals with autism of all ages. Recent research using advanced imaging technologies is consistently showing that brain development continues well at least into adolescence and early adulthood (e.g., Horska, Kaufmann, Brant, Naidu, Harris, Barker, 2002). Namely, Thompson, Giedd, Woods, MacDonald, Evans and Toga (2000) reported the creation of networks of growth patterns in the developing human brain in children aged 3–15 years, which seems to decline only after puberty. Sowell, Thompson, Tessner and Toga (2001) mapped continued post adolescent brain growth. Keshavan, Diwadkar, DeBellis, Dick, Kotwal, Rosenberg, Sweeney, Minshew andd Pettegrew (2002) assessed age-related changes in the size and signal intensity of the corpus callosum of individuals aged 7–32 years and found that signal intensity decreased during childhood and adolescence and stabilized in young adulthood. Furthermore they showed that the size of the corpus callosum increases through young adulthood indicating continuing maturation.
Based on the above mentioned empirical evidence, many scientific, government and professional agencies and organizations have concluded that ABA-based procedures represent best practices for individuals with autism, are highly recommended and should be publicly funded. Examples of such agencies and organizations in the United States and other countries (e.g. Canada, Australia, the UK, etc.) are the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Academies Press, the Association for Science in Autism Treatment, Autism Speaks, the Organization For Autism Research, the Surgeon General of the United States, the New York State Department of Health and other (Dillenburger, in press).
Taking into consideration the medium and long-term benefits for a community from the development of an individual’s skills and analyzing the cost benefits for tax-payers, local authorities, states and countries by effective interventions (Knapp, Romeo, & Beecham, 2009; Motiwala, Gupta, Lilly, Ungar, & Coyte, 2006). Any decision regarding funding interventions should be data-driven, should take into account ethical considerations and should guarantee that the best known scientific practice up to date is delivered to individuals needing it. An example of a cost-benefit analysis is the fact that in the USA savings of approximately $200,000 per child by the age of 22 years and $1,000,000 by the age of 55 years were registered following behaviour analytic intervention (Dillenburger, in press).
References
- Blew, P. F., Schwartz, I. S., & Luce, S. C. (1985). Teaching functional community skills to autistic children using nonhandicapped peer tutors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 337–342.
- Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior problems through functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126.
- Daly, E. J., III, & Martens, B. K. (1994). A comparison of three interventions for increasing oral reading performance: Application of the instructional hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 459–469.
- Delano, M. E. (2007). Improving written language performance of adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 345–351.
- Dillenburger, K. (in press). Why re-invent the wheel? A behavior analyst’s reflections on pedagogy for inclusion for students with learning and developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability.
- Dillenburger, K. (2011a). The emperor’s new clothes: Eclecticism in autism treatment. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1119–1128.
- Dillenburger, K. (2008). International Scholar lecture. Brock University, Canada. Families living with children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Experiences and service needs (April, 1, 2008).
- Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., Gallagher, S., & McElhinney, M. (2004). Parent education and home-based behaviour analytic intervention: An examination of parents’ perceptions of outcome. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 29, 119–130.
- Dounavi, A. (2011). Treating Vocal Stereotypy in a Child with Autism: Differential Reinforcement of Other Behavior and Sensory Integrative Therapy. European Journal of Behaviour Analysis, 12, 239–248.
- Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1992). An analysis of maintenance following functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(4), 777–794.
- Eikeseth, S. (2009). Outcome of comprehensive psycho-educational interventions for young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 158–178.
- Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P, Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E., Eikeseth, S., & Cross. (2009). Meta-Analysis of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention for Children With Autism. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38(3), 439–450.
- Gaylord-Ross, R. J., Haring, T. G., Breen, C., & Pitts-Conway, V. (1984). “The training and generalization of social interaction skills with autistic youth”. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 229–247.
- Gena, A., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Poulson, C. L. (1996). Training and generalization of affective behavior displayed by youth with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 291–304.
- Hagopian L.P, Adelinis J.D. (2001). Response blocking with and without redirection for the treatment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 527–530.
- Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of functional communication training with and without Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20,extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 211–235.
- Haring, T. G., Kennedy, C. H., Adams, M. J., & Pitts-Conway, V. (1987). Teaching generalization of purchasing skills across community settings to autistic youth using videotape modelling. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 89–96.
- Haring, T. G., Roger, B., Lee, M., Breen, C., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1986). Teaching social language to moderately handicapped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 159–171.
- Harris, S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (2000). Age and IQ at intake as predictors of placement for young children with autism: A four- to six-year follow-up. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 137–142.
- Horner R.D, Keilitz I. (1975). Training mentally retarded adolescents to brush their teeth. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 8, 301–309.
- Horska, A., Kaufmann, W. E., Brant, L. J., Naidu, S., Harris, J. C., Barker, P. B. (2002). In vivo quantitative proton MRSI study of brain development from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 15(2), 137–43.
- Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen,H.G., Green, G., & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 359–383.
- Keshavan, M. S., Diwadkar, V. A., DeBellis, M., Dick, E., Kotwal, R., Rosenberg, D. R., Sweeney, J. A., Minshew, N., Pettegrew, J. W. (2002). Development of the corpus callosum in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood. Life Science, 70(16), 1909–22.
- Knapp, M., Romeo, r., & Beecham, J. (2009).Economic cost of autism in the UK. Autism, 13, 317–336.
- Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. Journal of the Association for Persons With Severe Handicaps, 24, 174–185.
- Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. D. (1992). Improving social skills and disruptive behavior in children with autism through self-management. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 341–353.
- Krantz, P.J., & McClannahan, L.E. (1993). Teaching children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 121–132.
- Kuppens, S. & Onghena, P. (2011). Sequential meta-analysis to determine the sufficiency of cumulative knowledge: The case of early intensive behavioural intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 168–176, doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.04.002.
- Lalli, J. S., Mace, F. C., Wohn, T., & Livezey, K. (1995). Identification and modification of a response-class hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 551–559.
- McComas, J. J., Wacker, D. P., & Cooper, L. J. (1996). Experimental analysis of academic performance in a classroom setting. Journal of Behavioral Education, 6, 121–130.
- McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-term outcome for children with autism who received early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 359–372
- McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClannahan, L. E. (1983). A modified incidental-teaching procedure for autistic youth: Acquisition and generalization of receptive object labels. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 329–338.
- McMorrow, M. J., & Foxx, R. M. (1986). Some direct and generalized effects of replacing an autistic man’s echolalia with correct responses to questions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 289–297.
- Miller, N. & Neuringen, A. (2000). Reinforcing variability in adolescents with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 151–165.
- Monahan, S. L. & Bryer, F. (2004). Shadowing Techniques for Young Children with Autism: Extending ABA Procedures from Home to School. In: Bartlett, B. (Editor); Bryer, F. (Editor); Roebuck, D. (Editor). Educating: Weaving Research into Practice, 4, 34–46.
- Motiwala, S. S., Gupta, S., Lilly, M. B., Ungar, W. J., & Coyte, P. C. (2006). The Cost-Effectiveness of Expanding Intensive Behavioural Intervention to All Autistic Children in Ontario. Healthcare Policy, 1(2), 135–151.
- Osborne, L.A. & Reed, P. (2008). An evaluation of the role of reinforcement-based interventions in determining the effectiveness of ‘eclectic’ approaches to teaching children with autism spectrum disorders. Behavioural Development Bulletin, 14, 30- 39.
- Palmen, A., Didden, R, Arts, M. (2008). Improving question asking in high-functioning adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Effectiveness of small-group training. Autism, 12(1), 83–98.
- Reichow, B., & Wolery, M. (2009). Comprehensive synthesis of early intensive behavioural interventions for young children with autism based on the UCLA Young Autism Project Model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 23–41
- Schepis, M. M., Reid, D. H., Fitzgerald, J. R., Faw, G. D., VanDenPol, R. A., & Welty, P. (1982). A program for increasing manual signing by autistic and profoundly retarded youth within the daily environment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 363–379.
- Smith, M. D. & Coleman, D. (1986). Managing the behavior of adults with autism in the job setting. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 145–154.
- Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(22), 8819–29.
- Taylor, B. A. & Levin, L. (1998). Teaching a student with autism to make verbal initiations: effects of a tactile prompt. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 651–654.
- Thompson, P. M., Giedd, J. N., Woods, R. P., MacDonald, D., Evans, A. C., Toga, A. W. (2000). Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps, Nature, 404 (6774), 190–3.
- Thompson, R. H.. & Iwata. B. A. (2007). A Comparison of Outcomes from Descriptive and Functional Analyses of Problem Behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(2), 333–338.
- Zachor, D. A., Ben-Itschak, B., Rabinovich, A.-L., & Lahat, E. (2007). Change in autism core symptoms with intervention. Research on Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 304–317.
For the translation a heartfelt thank you to Anne Burzinski.
o
5. The Emperor’s new clothes: Eclecticism in autism treatment
Summary of the study
(Karola Dillenburger, 2011; Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder 5 (2011) 1119–1128, originally published online 22 January 2011; DOI 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.008)
Abstract
Although ABA is more and more recognized as the scientific way to go, many European governments prefer an eclectic model as they argue that it is more children-centred and pragmatic. This paper shows why ABA is truly practical and child-centred. This article shows how false information leads to less effective treatments being chosen rather than supporting evidence-based treatments such as ABA, which is actually uniform, practical and child-centric.
Introduction
This article first describes differences in government recommendations under A, then both the eclectic approach (see B) as well as ABA (see C) declared to then explore reasons for the different recommendations of governments (see D).
People with autism spectrum disorder are impaired in social interaction, their flexibility and in their behavior (APA, DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Autism can only be diagnosed until now, despite a wide range of research, if possible affected persons are observed in their behaviour and their reference persons are interviewed (Keenan, Dillenburger, Doherty, Byrne & Gallagher, 2010).
As the number of children with autism has risen dramatically (Fombonne, 2005). The costs are as high as 3.2 million $ per individual if he or she is not properly treated (CDC, 2010), That’s why it’s important to find out the most successful treatment method. Mental health and the quality of life of affected families can also suffer from autism (Dillenburger, Keenan, Doherty, Byrne, & Gallagher, 2010).
Even though, it is crucial to accept differences that will continue to exist, one must also educate and intervene further (Baron-Cohen, 2008; Helt et al., 2008; Jordan, 2008; Lamb, 2009; Markram, Rinaldi, & Markram, 2007). Although these differences must therefore be accepted, it is appropriate at the same time to continue to deal with them. Unesco Salamanca Statement (CSIE, 2010) shows that inclusion is the best possibility to show acceptance. How can children with autism learn skills that are required for social interaction (CSIE, 2010; Oxoby, 2009)?
There are many interventions that originally come from various professions (Archart-Treichel, 2010). Furthermore, there are parents who have developed their own intervention. Some of them are described in books some require exclusive training.
A: Recommendations from governments
Many governments have discussed the most successful interventions, results vary (NSP, 2009, Mudford et al., 2009; Task Group on Autism, 2002; Task Force on Autism, 2001; Dunlop et al., 2009; Weinmann et al., 2009; Perry & Condillac, 2003). The recommendations are different. Some recommend treatments based on ABA, some prefer treatment to be eclectic.
Government recommendations in North America
Clinical and social results, as well as financial efficiency in North America suggest that interventions based on principles of behavior are the way to go (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). They justify their decision with clinical and social evidence (Howar, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, &Amp; Stanislaw, 2005; Reichow & Wolery, 2009; Zachor, Ben-Ichak, Rabinovich & Lahat, 2007); Foster & Mash, 1999) and the financial effectiveness of the method (Knapp et al., 2007). Systematic reviews of research literature demand that health insurances cover diagnosis and treatment of autism, including ABA (ATAA, 2010). Many states have already signed it while others are still waiting for introduction. The same is true for Canada. Many US states have already complied with this demand, and more are about to be introduced. A similar situation exists in canada.
Recommendations from European governments
The governments in most of Europe, except Norway, promote the eclectic approach which allows for a range of interventions, as there is no definitive evidence that supports one treatment over the other (Task Force Autism, 2001, p.117). What is the eclectic approach? Many different interventions are selected from different available interventions, according to identified needs (Gladwell 2010). The approach is viewed as flexible and child-centred. Therefore, it is financially supported in Europe.
B: The eclectic approach
Why is this child-centric and pragmatic approach now being criticized with various treatment options? Why isn’t the eclectic approach also advocated in North America?
In order to understand this and the consequence of the eclectic approach, various points of view need to be examined:
- What to think about:
An eclectic model integrates new methods, but it won’t develop them themselves.
- Are eclectic treatments successful?
Some methods in an eclectic model are evidence-based; some might even be controversial (Jacobson et al, 2005; Perry, 2000; Perry & Condillac, 2003; Tweed, Connolly, & Beaulieu, 2009).
- What is the benefit of a combined treatment method?
Whether a combination of different approaches is more promising than the sole application of individual approaches is questionable. Howard et al (2005) found, however, that the application of a form of teaching based on ABA is more promising than a treatment that is eclectic (Osborne & Reed, 2008).
- What are the theoretical foundations on which eclectic approaches are based?
Some of the treatment approaches have a theoretical basis; However, this often contradicts each other. Therefore, an eclectic approach has no common conclusive theoretical basis. - Can staff learn how to use eclectic treatments? Since the successful approaches often require a master’s degree and are very extensive, a person cannot possibly be sufficiently proficient in all treatment methods.
C: Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA)
Proponents of the eclectic approach often say ABA should be integrated into an eclectic model, but see ABA as too one-sided in itself. This is because they often equate ABA with a particular method, such as discrete trial training or treatment according to Lovaas.
What is ABA?
“In mact, ABA is not a specific method of treatment but” a scientific approach that seeks to uncover environmental conditions that reliably influence socially significant behaviors and, based on this, a method of behavioural change. Developed that practically implements these discoveries. ” (Cooper et al., 2007, p3). ABA can be described as a way of teaching. ABA has shown that it can bring about significant behavioural changes in both Individuals and the Group.
Once ABA is applied, knowledge of the basics of behavior is used. Since one cannot behave (even feelings are and bhavior), knowledge of behavior should be used to address socially relevant behavior (Baer, Wolf, & Risley, 1968; Newman, 1992; 4. 1998).
For this to happen, the target behavior must first be defined. This is done by talking to the person concerned and their caregivers. The target behaviour is socially or pedagogically important (Lamb, 2009). It is always appropriate and individually tailored for each child, also curricula may be used as a base (ACE, 2011). Second, the function of behavior is analyzed by looking for contingencies, therefore, appropriate interventions can be developed (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994). As a result, effective teaching strategies can be created. Afterwards, a baseline is established and intervention is introduced while more data is collected to be able to ensure the effectiveness of intervention or to be able to adjust them to new requirements. As the lessons are introduced, Data will continue to be collected to assess effectiveness or adjust the teaching accordingly.
Once the behavior has been learned by the person with autism, the goal is to generalize it. This means that the person with autism learns to show the newly acquired behavior in other people, in other situations and over long periods of time.
As ABA is built on the basis of behaviour and individually tailored to the needs of children, the number of possible teaching strategies is unlimited. However, some of them, such as discrete trial teaching or natural environment teaching, are particularly popular as they have proven to be reliably successful. All teaching methods have in common that they are based on a functional analysis of the behavior and are individually tailored to each child (Iwata et al., 1982/1994)
Is ABA effective?
There exist a lot of scientific papers about the evidence of ABA (Eldevik et al., 2010) and there are also more and more meta-analyses. Some people who are favoring the eclectic approach claim nevertheless that one can’t tell the effectiveness of ABA without Randomised Controlled Trials (RCT). RCTs are also referred to as the gold standard of studies due to clear statements on a question. At the same time, however, these advocates do not require RCTs for the eclectic approach (Morris, 2009). At the same time they don’t demand RCTs for the eclectic approach (Keenan & Dillenburger, 2011). But RCTs were designed to test drugs not medical procedures. So it is difficult to demand them. Nonetheless, there are some RCTs that have compared ABA with parts of eclectic treatments ” ABA was more effective in all of these in changing a range of behaviors (Birdnbrauer & leach, 1993; Cohen, Amarine-Dickens, & Smith, 2006; Eikesetz, 2009; Eikesetz, Smith, Jahr & Eldevik, 2007; Eldevik et al., 2009, 2010; Howard et al., 2005; Magiati, Charman, & Howlin, 2007; Rogers & Vismara, 2008; Sheinkopf & Siegel, 1998; Smith, Groen & Wynn, 2000; Zachor et al., 2007).
When using ABA-based procedures it is necessary to include a well qualified behavior analyst to plan a child-centred and individualized treatment. Behavior Analysis is a profession that is recognized internationally (BACB, 2010).
Criticism from proponents of the eclectic approach to ABA
Despite all of that, ABA is still criticized. It’s said that ABA is a single approach that is inflexible and rigid in comparison to an eclectic approach (McConkey 2007). his opinion is based on misinterpretation and bad knowledge about ABA (Jordan 2001) that doesn“t consider that ABA is a unified parsimonious approach that is flexible, individualized and firmly rooted in data-based, scientific research evidence.
D: Reasons for differences in reports and guideline
When a child is diagnosed with Autism, parents are faced with a “forced choice”. Either they go for ABA, which is seen as to rigid (Jordan, 2008) or as best practise (Chiesa, 2005). Or they mix different interventions together in an eclectic approach, which is seen as more flexible (McConkey et al., 2007) or inconsistent and ineffective (Howard et al, 2005).
A reason for different opinions about which treatment to use is more often based on “work experience” than on scientific findings (DfES, 2002; NIASA 2003). But when spending that amount of taxpayers” money it should be best practice to base decisions on research than on opinions. And if experts are included in the decision making they should be highly knowledgeable. Unfortunately, all of the reports from European governments are written without consulting adequately qualified experts in ABA. The lack of that was pointed out several times, without resulting effective change (Gladwell 2010, Mattaini, 2008; PEAT, 2008). Therefore, guidelines are inaccurate.
Only the Scottish government (Dunlop et al., 2009) reacted on this kind of criticism (about inaccurate and out-dated description of ABA, as well as mentioning no recent research papers) from parents so far. And it has withdrawn their initial recommendations. Now they revise their report with the help of a well-known behavior analyst.
Category mistakes
ABA is not one specific kind of intervention. Instead it is a whole amount of different approaches based on principles of behavior. Unfortunately, many European governments make the mistake to consider ABA to be one method (Chiesa, 2005; Cooper et al., 2007; Dillenburger & Keenan, 2009. In fact ABA is a child-centred and pragmatic approach, which could also be called eclectic (Leidermann, 2010) as a behavior analyst uses a “broad range of interventions resources and develops and adjusts individually tailored additional interventions on the basis of continuous data collection.” In reality it isn’t called eclectic as ABA is not applying interventions by chance, but is to be planned carefully. Selected interventions are based on continuously recorded data to be able to adjust to momentarily problems.
Abstract
Autismus-spectrum disorder and its treatment has since often been researched, a lot of money has been spent. Even if there is a lot of research about Autism Spectrum Disorder and its treatment and even if a lot of money is spent, there are studies who report that parents are twice as likely to experience psychological problems (Keenan et al, 2002), stress (Burrows 2010) as well as some uncertainty about which treatment to use (Lamb, 2009).
Why governments don’t admit flaws
So why don’t governments admit flaws and mistake in their reports for the sake of parents and children? The reason might be, that it is difficult to admit that a lot of time and money and belief has been spent on less effective treatment in the past, or that it is difficult to admit a marginal knowledge of behavior analysis.
This paper tried to reveal reasons for different opinions from American and European governments regarding the most effective treatment of autism. When paying more attention on these reasons in the future it might be possible to deliver best treatment possible for children with autism. And therefore, to increase the quality of life for affected families as well as to decrease associated costs (decreasing the amount of help required in special school by treating a child with ABA, mounted to 208 500$ per child for 18 years) (Chasson et al., 2007).
Summing up, the eclectic approach is a fancy name for pseudoscientific ways of working, ABA is science.
To read the full study, please click here.
For the permission to post this study we thank Dr Karola Dillenburger BCBA‑D; Clinical Psychologist (HPC) .
o
6. Normal Peer Models and children with autism’s learning
Summary of the study
(Andrew L. Egel, Gina S. Richman, and Robert L. Koegel , Normal Peer Models and autistic
children´s learning, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 141,3–12, Nr. 1, Spring 1981)
Background
As federal legislation has set up passages asking for education in least restrictive environment. there is considerable research regarding the possibility of integrating children with autism in a “normal school” (Russo & Koegel, 1977). Can children with autism benefit from the integration by successfully modelling their peers without disabilities? There is a vast variety of research demonstrating that peer models lead to changes in the behavior of children without autism (Elliot & Vasta, 1970; Hartup & Coates, 1967; Igelmo, 1976; Kobasigawa, 1968; Miran, 1975; Bandura & Kupers, 1964; Clark, 1965; Debus, 1970; Ridberg, Parke & Hetherington, 1971; Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967; Bandura & menlove, 1968).
Therefore, it might be considered that similar results are true for children with disabilities. Several studies showed that responses could be brought under stimulus control of neuro-typical children (Apolloni, Cooke, & Cooke, 1976; Barry & overman, 1977; Peterson, Peterson, & Scriven, 1977; Rauer, Cooke, & Apolloni, 1978; Talkington, Hall, & Altman, 1973). Es wurde ebenfalls untersucht, inwiefern es hilfreich ist, wenn Kinder mit Autismus solche ohne Autismus beobachten.The effectiveness of children with autism observing others has been studied as well. A case history by Coleman and Stedman (1974) described the successful modelling of voice loudness and increased labelling vocabulary.
Other studies couldn’t affirm this finding and suggested that it cannot be held for all types of children with autism. E.g. stimulus over-selectivity in low-functioning children with autism may account for a failure in learning through observation. This might get less for higher-level children with autism (Varni, Lovaas, Koegel & Everett, 1979).
This study tested if at least some children with autism benefit from exposure to neuro-typical peers.
Method
Subjects
4 children with autism between 5–10 years (mental age 3–5 years) took part in this study. They made general overall progress but had difficulties in acquiring certain tasks in their classroom curriculum.
Not all of them were able to speak and most of them produced echolalia. All of them showed low to moderate amount of self-stimulatory behavior and some threw tantrums. Most of them had considerable problems in appropriate play, social behavior and self-help skills.
Peer models
Three neuro-typical children from neighbouring classrooms, as well as one very high functioning child with autism (Child 3, Task 2), who weren’t older than two years of their attributed subjects, were selected as role models as they could answer all tasks correctly and were responding to adults request.
Setting
The experiment took place in an area of the classroom. Sessions took between five and fifteen minutes with 10–40 trials per session. There was a session at least every three days and with a maximum of two sessions per day. The second author, and an in behavior modification experienced undergraduate who didn’t know about the hypothesis of this study were the therapists.
Target behavior
The target behavior was some activity from the curriculum the child had difficulty in acquiring (discriminating between two colors, shapes, use of prepositions, affirmative yes/no picture).
Design
Baseline (no modeling)
The baseline was measured through the therapist asking the child to perform a task (e.g. “give me the circle”, “give me blue”). Correct responses were reinforced (e.g. “good boy”) while incorrect responses were followed by a verbal “no”. If the child was incorrect for about three successive trials, prompt fading procedures (e.g. manually guiding the child’s hand) were used.
Modeling conditions
Identical teaching procedures as in baseline, but the therapist worked first with the model child who responded correctly and who got reinforced immediately. The model sat beside the child with autism or opposite the child who was instructed to look at the stimulus material. The child with autism was asked to look at the work material, while the “model child” solved the tasks correctly and was immediately praised as a result. Afterwards, the therapist presented the same stimuli material and instruction to the child with autism. Consequences for incorrect answers were the same as in baseline. Task counted as acquired if the child answered correct 8 out of 10 times without need of prompting.
Additional no model trials
The procedure was the same as in baseline. The reason for this trial was to control if the child with autism would answer consequently correct on 30 trials without a model two days later.
Data recording and reliability
Each test run was assessed by the therapist as correct or incorrect. The therapist scored the answer and an independent observer monitored the score. There was a reliability of 100%.
Reesult
Baseline showed very low levels of correct responding (24–50% correct responding (50% equals chance level)). Correct Responses increased very fast in modelling condition. The children achieved 8 out of 10 correct answers after a maximum of 20 trials. Some of them even achieved 100% correct answers (10 out of 10).
These results were successfully repeated at the no-modelling condition. The percentage stabilised or even increased at the rate of model condition.
Discussion
Peer modelling increased performance on discrimination tasks for participating children with autism.
Limitations
Children with autism differ in pre-teaching and in developmental level. Participating children were not as deeply impaired than that in Varni et al. (1997) study who were at the level of severe retardation and had high self-stimulatory behavior. Participating children in contrast were good in imitating, had large receptive language abilities and were acquiring a small functional expressive vocabulary. Therefore, it might be necessary to expose children with autism to some pre-teaching before using peer modelling.
Participating children had an intelligence quotient of 50–87 that might be the precondition for benefitting from modelling. A lot of children with autism function at that level.
Abstract
Similarity of peer models to learner
Children with autism might benefit from same age children compared to adults (Barry & Overmann, 1977; Hicks 1965; Kazdin 1974; Kornhaber & Schroeder 1975). Age and sex of the model and the observer may influence the probability of the model being imitated (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963; Hartup & Lougee, 1975; Rosekrans 1967). Therefore, learning didn’t take place when adults were modelling (baseline).
Novelty
Modelling children was novel to the children with autism and this kind of learning might have increased the saliency of the required responses and reinforcer. When it is difficult to direct children with autism responding to relevant cues, this type of teaching might therefore be a possibility for the children.
Classroom implications
The results show that it may be effective to put moderately impaired children with autism into classrooms with peers without autism (two children of the study are in a normal classroom in the meantime and seem to keep on imitating peers).
The language ability, affects of children with autism on normal peers, overall functioning level, teacher knowledge of applying modification techniques may influence successful integration and need to be studied therefore.
As modelling peers seems to be an effective way to learn for children with autism, it has to be evaluated deeper in future.
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
To read the full study, please download the orginal study from JABA.
Thanks a lot for allowing this study to be summarized, translated and published to: Kathy Hill, business manager of JABA
For the summary and the translation a heartfelt thank you to Caroline Diziol.
o
7. Using Aberrant Behaviors as reinforcers for children with autism
Summary of the study
(Marjorie H. Charlop, Patricia F. Kurtz, Fran Greenberg Casey, veröffentlicht in JABA Summer 1990, 239 163–181, Nr. 2 Using Aberrant Behaviors as reinforcers for autistic children)
Background
Finding reinforcers for children with autism can be difficult as they are often not interested in toys or social reinforcers. Using food as reinforcement can be problematic because of difficulties in administration and satiation.
Aberrant behaviors as stereotypy (Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons and Long 1972) are a highly preferred activity and might therefore be used as a reinforcer (Premack 1959). First studies showed initial support (Hung 1978, Sugai and White 1986, Wolery, Kirk and Gast 1985). No negative side effects such as an increase of stereotypical behavior. (Wolery 1985)
Many children with autism spectrum disorder also exhibit delayed echolalia (Lovaas, Varni, Koegel, Lorsch, 1977, Prizant and Rydell, 1984) and perseverative behavior (Epstein, Taumban, Lovaas, 1985; Lovaas, Newsom, Hickman, 1987) that may possess reinforcing functions.
This study that consists of 3 experiments evaluates the efficacy of using aberrant behavior (stereotypy, delayed echolalia, perseverate behavior) as reinforcers. The study also considers possible negative side effects as the increase of aberrant behavior in detail.
Experiment 1 compares:
- the use of stereotypy
- with the use of food/edibles and conditions of
- varied consequences (food or stereotypy) (as Egel 1981 suggested that using more than one reinforcer in a varied format might raise their effectiveness).
Experiment 2 compares
- the use of delayed echolalia
- with the use of food/edibles and conditions of
- conditions of varied consequences (food or delayed echolalia).
Experiment 3 compares the use of perseverate behavior
- with the use of
- with the use of food/edibles and conditions of
- and conditions of varied consequences (food or perseverate behavior.)
Method
Subjects
Participants were diagnosed as autistic. They attended biweekly therapy session. They attended at an after school behavior modification program for a least six months. They were described as unmotivated to learn (and engaging in aberrant behavior).
Setting and Tasks
The room had several toys and educational stimuli. The room could be seen by observers through a one-way mirror. Every child got three tasks that were from their curriculum and that they haven’t had mastered for several months.
Design
The effectiveness of the reinforcers (food, aberrant behavior, varied consequences) was assessed in a multi-element design. Every experimental condition was presented at the most three times one after another. In addition, the total number of meetings was varied in order to measure changes in performance over time.
Design
Baseline
The selected tasks were presented in a typical 15 min work session. Baseline was collected over a 6 ? to 8 month period (experiment 1 & 2) and in the weeks before the experiment 3 for 1–2 times per week.
Experimental conditions
Every child had two 15 min experimental sessions per week (2–5 days apart). The experimenter sat opposite the child and presented tasks when he got eye contact and the child was sitting attentively. The order of the presentation varied. When a child gave the correct response, the experimenter reinforced with praise AND the chosen consequence. When a child gave the incorrect response or didn’t respond within 5 s, he presented a verbal “No”. A correction trial was presented after two consecutive incorrect trials, these were not included in the data analysis. The experimenter recorded the answer of the child after each task. Beyond that the occurrences of the aberrant behavior was recorded.
Consequence condition
Direct observations and discussions with parents and therapists revealed child specific aberrant behavior that was chosen as consequence.
- Food: The child could choose from preferred food items Aberrant behavior: The child was allowed to engage in aberrant behavior (stereotype, delayed echolalia, perseverance behavior) for 3–5 seconds after a correct answer. The child was prompted, if necessary Varied consequence: Child could choose food or aberrant behavior.
Experimental Observation
After observers training there was an observer behind the one-way mirror who counted the occurrence of the aberrant behavior (besides of the aberrant condition) stereotype and off-task behaviors with a 10 second partial interval scoring procedure.
Post-experimental session observation
The child was observed in a 15 min post-session observation either in another work session with a different therapist or in a free play situation.
Reliability
Inter-rater reliability was calculated for at least 33 % of baseline and experimental sessions as well as for stereotypy and (off task) behavior.
Experiment 1
Method
Subjects
4 autistic boys between 6 and 9 years (mental age between 2 and 4 years) took part in the experiment. All of them were at least minimal verbal and showed different stereotypy, off-task behavior, tantrums and aggression.
Tasks and Procedure
The children had 3 different tasks to master and received three consequence conditions (food, stereotype, varied).
Additional analysis
One child was chosen for more detailed analysis. He (and his aggression?) got observed for 30 instead of 15 minutes. His trained mother observed his stereotypy also at home before and during the experiment. Inter-rated reliability was between 92 and 100 %.
Results and discussion
The most effective reinforcer for all the children was stereotypy. The food consequence was the least successful and was even below the baseline for one child. The varied consequence was also reinforcing for 2 children.
When comparing the number of stereotypy, it didn’t increase during sessions with stereotypy as a consequence and was less for two children compared to the food conditions. This is also true for the post-session observations where stereotypy increased less for stereotypy consequences than for food. Additional analysis of one child showed that aggression and off-task behavior increased in post-session observation. This might be the reason as stereotypy decreased aggression in the aberrant behavior condition, increasing task-performance at the same time. The mother’s data showed that there were no side effects and that the stereotypy behavior decreased at home. The decrease in stereotypical behaviour at home must be viewed with caution, as this trend could be seen even before the experiment began. However, the mother said she had not introduced any innovations that could explain this trend.
Experiment 1 showed that stereotypy as a reinforcer was more effective than food and than varied consequences. Furthermore, it did not show any side effects but might have decreased inappropriate behavior.
Experiment 2
Delayed echolalia seems to have reinforcing qualities (Lovaas et al, 1977; Prizant & Rydell, 1984) and was therefore examined.
Method
Subjects and Tasks
3 verbal boys between 8 and 10 years participated in this experiment and had to work on three (one boy on two) tasks. Therefore, work session took 10–15 minutes each. The procedure was the same as of experiment 1, except that the consequence was the encouragement of delayed echolalia instead of stereotypy. Inter-rater-reliability was between 99–100%.
Results and discussion
Delayed echolalia was the most effective reinforcer for two children. But the varied condition was very similar. One child had slightly better results for varied condition than for delayed echolalia. Food was also slightly effective more effective than baseline for two children and brought worse effects than the baseline for one child.
There was hardly any difference between stereotypy and off-task behavior as well as delayed echolalia in experimental sessions compared to post experimental work sessions.
One child had increases in stereotypy and off-task behavior in the free play situation another had increases in delayed echolalia in the free play situation. This might have occurred because of the lack of structure and supervision in free play, as it didn’t appear to be a function of a particular experimental condition. Detailed analysis for one child had similar results. There was a decrease in delayed echolalia at home. This is a trend that started before treatment and must be interpreted with caution.
This experiment also showed that delayed echolalia may act as an effective reinforcer.
Experiment 3
The effectiveness of perseveration with specific object as a reinforcer was compared to edibles and stereotypy (instead of varied).
Method
Subjects
3 autistic boys between 6 and 10 (mental age 3 to 5) took part in the experiment. They were allowed to access the perseverated object for 3 to 5 seconds upon a correct response, detailed data collection took place from all parents. All three boys were continued to be watched at home by their trained parents.
Inter-rater-reliability was between 93 and 98 %.
Results and discussion
Perseverate behavior was the most effective reinforcer. For some children, stereotypy was similar effective. Food was close or beneath the baseline.
As in experiment 1 and 2, there were no negative side effects. The researched behavior increased in the beginning, but decreased below baseline later on.
General discussion
Aberrant behavior could be shown as effective reinforcers for difficult task. Using aberrant behavior as a reinforcer doesn’t seem to have negative side effects (see results of them at home and after session).
Stereotypy has some sensory and perceptual properties (rincover and newsom, 1985) that may serve as primary reinforcers as they might stimulate the central nervous system (Lovaas et al 1987).
The same might be true for delayed echolalia and perseverate behavior as shown by Rincover 1978 and Rincover et al (1979) found reinforcing visual, auditory and proprior-receptive sensory consequences that maintain stereotypic behavior in children with autism.
When children with autism get older, their low-level stereotypy is replaced by perseveration and echolalic speech (Epstein et al 1985). Therefore Lovaas (1987) and Epstein (1985) suggested that the same patterns may result in maintaining the latter behavior. (they are repetitive, complex stimuli are produced, and they interfere with appropriate behavior).
Abstract
The experiment showed that instead of trying to eliminate aberrant behavior, the reinforcing properties of them might be used to result in better learning.
And as the experiment showed, there were no negative side effects. The children even accepted the taking away of the object quite easily and were eager to work. This might therefore be a good alternative in behavior change programs.
Limitations of the study are that some children with autism show hardly any stereotypical behavior, or behavior that is very difficult to control by the therapist. No long-term effects have been observed.
Nevertheless it might be a good method to motivate children in the future.
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
To read the full study, please download the original study from JABA.
Thanks a lot for allowing this study to be summarized, translated and published to: Kathy Hill, business manager of JABA
For the summary and the translation a heartfelt thank you to Caroline Diziol.
o
8. Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism.
Summary of the study
(Geraldine Dawson, Sally Rogers, Jeffrey Munson, Milani Smith, Jamie Winter, Jessica Greenson, Amy Donaldson and Jennifer Varley Pediatrics 2010;125;e17-e23; originally published online Nov 30, 2009; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2009–0958)
Background:
In 1987 Lovaas’s study on an early behavioral intervention for children with autism found that 49% of the participants were able to attend a regular class room setting in a mainstream school and had made significant IQ gains. This finding led to an increased interest into the effects of early intervention and raised questions about early plasticity in children with autism. Despite subsequent early intervention studies which have found gains in IQ for a subgroup of children, questions regarding the efficacy of early intervention persist, due to lack of methodological rigor. Authors of meta-analysis of efficacy of early behavioral intervention argue that stronger evidence that early behavioral interventions yield better outcomes than standard care is required.
Aims of the study:
This study, The Early Start Denver Model, was a randomized controlled trial of early intensive behavioral intervention. The authors hypothesized that the early intervention would result in significant improvements in cognitive abilities of young children with autism. There were three major differences in this study in comparison to previous early behavioral intervention studies. The first was that a high level of methodological rigor. This includes gold- standard diagnostic criteria, randomization, comprehensive outcome measures conducted by naive examiners and measures of fidelity of implementation of a manualized intervention was maintained. The second was that all children who took part in the study were less than 30 months old at entry. Thirdly was a comprehensive early behavioral intervention for infants to preschool-aged children with ASD that integrates applied behavior analysis (ABA) with developmental and relationship-based approaches. The intervention was provided in a toddler’s natural environment (the home) and was delivered by trained therapists and parents. In our study, children received structured intervention at high intensity.
Patients and Methods:
Study Procedures:
Forty-eight children between 18 and 30 months of age, diagnosed with autistic disorder or pervasive developmental disorder (PDD), not otherwise specified (NOS), were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 groups: (1) the ESDM group received yearly assessments, 20 hours week of the ESDM intervention parent training, and parent delivery for 5 or more hours/week of ESDM, in addition to whatever community services as chosen by the parents (2) the assess-and-monitor (A/M) group received yearly assessments with intervention recommendations and referrals for intervention from commonly available community providers in the greater Seattle region.
Participants:
Participants were recruited through pediatrics centers, hospitals pre-schools and local autism organizations. Inclusion criteria included age below 30 months at entry, met the criteria for autism diagnosis and willingness to participate in a two year intervention.
Measures:
Autism diagnostic measures included; the autism diagnostic interview-revised, autism diagnostic observation schedule, mullen sacles of early learning, vineland adaptive behavior scales and repetitive behavior scale.
Randomization:
Participants were placed into 2 groups on the basis of IQ and gender to ensure comparable IQ and gender ratios between groups. The intervention groups did not differ at baseline in severity of autism symptoms based on ADOS scores, chronological age, IQ, gender, or adaptive behaviors.
Intervention Groups:
ESDM Group:
The ESDM group was provided with intervention by trained therapists for 2‑hour sessions, twice per day, 5 days/ week, for 2 years. A detailed intervention manual and curriculum were used. One or both parents were trained from the primary therapist twice monthly on the principles and specific techniques of the ESDM were taught. Parents were asked to ESDM teaching strategies during daily activities and to keep track of the number of hours during which they used these strategies. Teaching strategies are consistent principles of ABA, such as the use of operant conditioning, shaping, and chaining. Each child’s plan is individualized. There is a strong parent-family role responsive to each family’s unique characteristics. The programs were supervised graduate-level therapists who had a minimum of 5 years experience providing early intervention for young children with autism. On-going consultation was provided by clinical psychologist, speech-language pathologist, and developmental behavioral pediatrician. An occupational therapist provided consultation as needed. The intervention was delivered was delivered by therapists who typically held a bachelors degree, received 2 months of training from the lead therapist and met weekly with the lead therapist.
A/M Group:
Children who were randomly assigned to the A/M group received comprehensive diagnostic evaluations, intervention recommendations, and community referrals at baseline and again at each of the 2 follow-up assessments. Families were provided with resource and reading materials at baseline and twice a year throughout the study. The children received an average of 9.1 hours of 1:1 therapy and average 9.3 hours per week of group intervention across the 2‑year period during which the intervention study was conducted.
Data analysis:
The effect of ESDM intervention was assessed by using repeated-measures analysis of variance, baseline scores with 1- and 2‑year outcome scores. The primary outcome measures were the MSEL and the VABS composite standard scores.
Secondary outcome measures were the ADOS severity score, the RBS, MSEL, and VABS subscale scores, and changes in diagnostic status (autistic disorder, PDD NOS, and no diagnosis).
Results:
No serious adverse effects related to the intervention were reported during the 2‑year period.
Year 1 Outcome:
The ESDM group demonstrated an average IQ increase of 15.4 points compared with an increase of 4.4 points in the A/M group. The ESDM group improved 17.8 points on receptive language compared with a 9.8‑point improvement in the A/M group. The groups did not differ in terms of adaptive behavior. The groups did not differ in terms of their ADOS severity scores or RBS total score after 1 year of intervention.
Year 2 Outcome:
Two years after the baseline assessment, the ESDM group showed significantly improved cognitive ability. The ESDM and A/M groups significantly differed in terms of their adaptive behavior. The ESDM group showed similar standard scores at the 1- and 2‑year outcomes, indicating a steady rate of development, whereas the A/M group, on average, showed an 11.2‑point average decline. Thus, the A/M group’s delays in overall adaptive behavior became greater when compared with the normative sample.
The A/M group showed average declines in standard scores that were twice as great as those in the ESDM group in the domains of socialization, daily living skills, and motor skills. The groups did not differ in terms of their ADOS severity scores or RBS total score after 2 years of intervention.
Diagnosis:
At intake the diagnosis in each group were not significantly different. After two years the diagnosis improved for 29.2% of the children in the ESDM group but only improved for one (4.8%) child in the A/M group. However, the diagnosis changed from PDD NOS at baseline to autistic disorder at year 2 for 2 (8.3%) children in the ESDM group and 5 (23.8%) children in the A/M group. Thus, children who received ESDM were significantly more likely to have improved diagnostic status at the 2‑year outcome compared with children in the A/M group.
Conclusion:
The outcomes of this study, which involve an increase in IQ scores of 17 points and significant gains in language and adaptive behavior, compare favorably with other controlled studies of intensive early intervention which delivered discrete trial intervention.
Link to original study click here
For the permission to post this study, great thanks to: American Academy of Pediatrics: Brad Rysz
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
For the translation a heartfelt thank you to Miri Zoller.
o
9. Outcome of comprehensive psycho-educational interventions for young children with autism
Summary of the study
(Svein Eikeseth, Akershus University College)
Aims of the study:
This study evaluated the outcomes of twenty-five comprehensive-psycho educational research papers on early intervention for children with autism. Of these twenty-five studies, three studies were about behavioral treatment, two studies evaluated TEACCH, and two studies evaluated the Colorado Health Sciences project.
Search Method:
Three search methods were used to identify the relevant outcomes of all the studies. The first method was the use of electronic search engines. The second method inspected recent publications to confirm that the search engines had identified the most recent studies. Finally, researchers known to be involved with the studies were contacted via email and asked to produce references of published and in press articles.
Criteria for assigning scientific merit:
Outcome of these studies were graded according to their scientific value and the magnitude of results documented. Scientific merit was evaluated based on: diagnosis, study design, dependent variables and treatment fidelity. Four levels were used to describe scientific merit:
Level 1:
This represented the highest possible rating.
Diagnosis:
A level 1 was assigned if the participants were diagnosed according to current international standards, which includes the use of the ICD-10 and the DSM-IV. Also, the diagnosis must have been set by clinicians who were independent of the study, or the diagnosis must have been based on well-researched diagnostic instruments including ADI‑R.
Study Design:
A level 1 was given to the design of the study if a randomized design was employed, that is, if participants have been assigned randomly to two or more study groups.
Dependent Variables:
A level 1 was assigned if intake and outcome measures assessed both intellectual and adaptive functioning. The instruments used to assess intellectual and adaptive functioning must be normalized and standardized. The IQ score must be derived from both language / communication skills as well as visual spatial or performance skills. In addition, to ensure objectiveness of the assessments, blind or independent assessors must have conducted the assessments.
Treatment Fidelity:
A level 1 was assigned to treatment fidelity if it was (a) directly assessed, or (b) treatment was described in treatment manuals.
Level 2:
This represented a moderate scientific merit.
Criteria for achieving Level 2 scientific merit was identical to that of Level 1 except that the study design was not random, so each participant did not have an equal chance of being placed in either of the study groups. Group assignment would be based on, for example, participants’ geographical location, parental choice, or availability of treatment personnel. These are examples of non-random group designs.
Level 3:
This represented a low scientific merit.
Diagnosis:
A level 3 was given for diagnosis if the diagnosis (based on the ICD-10 or DSM-IV criteria) was not blind or independent; or the diagnosis was not based on diagnostic instruments, or if the diagnosis was independent or blind but not based on ICD-10 or DSM-IV (or DSM-III for older studies); or if the study failed to specify which diagnostic system was used.
Study Design:
A level 3 was given to retrospective (archival) studies with comparison groups, or singlecase experimental studies where outcome measures were assessed pre and post.
Dependent Variables:
A level 3 was assigned when intake and outcome measures did not assess both intellectual and adaptive functioning, or measures were not normalized and standardized.
Treatment Fidelity:
Level 3 was given to treatment fidelity if insufficient assessment of treatment fidelity, or treatment not based on treatment manuals
Insufficient Scientific Value — ISV:
This was assigned to studies where the evidentiary support was so low that outcome data gave insufficient scientific meaning.
Criteria for deciding magnitude of results:
As with evaluating scientific merit, four levels were provided to evaluate the magnitude of the treatment effects. Once again, Level 1 represented the highest possible rating, and Level 4 represented the lowest rating.
Level 1:
Level 1 was given for the magnitude of results, if significant group differences on IQ and adaptive functioning (deviation or ratio scores) were reported. In addition Level 1 was given, if the assessment included measures of empathy, personality, school performance, friendship, and information regarding diagnostic changes.
Level 2:
Level 2 status was provided for significant group differences on either IQ or adaptive functioning (deviation or ratio scores). For both Level 1 and 2, the IQ measure must be based on language/communication skills in addition to visual spatial or performance skills.
Level 3:
Level 3 status was provided for significant group differences on developmental (or mental) age, or significant group differences on assessment instruments that are not normalized standardized (or significant group differences on improvement).
Stufe 4:
Level 4 studies reported significant pre-post improvements. In this review, only Levels 1–3 scientific evidence studies are evaluated according to magnitude of treatment effect. Studies classified with insufficient scientific value are excluded because for methodological reasons, they did not allow reliable conclusions regarding outcome to be drawn.
Classification of studies based on scientific merit and magnitude of results:
For information on each approach: TEACCH, The Denver Model and Applied Behavior Analysis, please download the PDF from this website (see link on bottom of this page).
Level 1 Scientific Merit:
Of the three models only one study received a level 1 for scientific merit. This was conducted by Smith, Groen and Wynn (2000) and was designed to evaluate ABA treatment. Results showed that the ABA treatment group scored significantly higher as compared to the parent training control group on intelligence, visual-spatial skills, language and academics, though not adaptive functioning. However, as the study did not show a significant group difference on adaptive functioning, it received a level 2 rating for magnitude of results.
Level 2 Scientific Merit:
Of the twenty-five studies that were evaluated, only four level 2 studies were identified and were all based on ABA treatment (Cohen, Amerine-Dickens, & Smith, 2006; Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eledevik, 2002, 2007; Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, Stanislaw, 2005; Remington et al., 2007). Three of the studies showed that the participants in the ABA treatment groups scored significantly higher on intelligence, language and adaptive functioning as compared to comparison group children (Cohen et al., 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2002, 2007; Howard et al., 2005). As a result these studies received a level 1 for the magnitude of the results. The Remington et al. study found that children in that ABA treatment group scored significantly higher as compared to children in the comparison group on intelligence, but not on adaptive functioning and language (as measured by standard scores). Therefore, this study received Level 2 fir the magnitude of the results rating. All four studies gained Level 2 scientific merit classification because they lacked a randomized study design: three studies (Cohen et al., Howard et al., Remington et al.) based group assignment on parental preference.
Level 3 Scientific Merit:
Eleven of the twenty-five studies received a level 3 rating. Two studies are based on the TEACCH model; (Mukaddes, Kaynak, Kinali, Besikci, & Issever, 2004; Ozonoff & Cathcart, 1998) and both studies received Level 3 on the magnitude of the results rating. Ozonoff and Cathcart did not specify which diagnostic system the children’s diagnosis was based on, whether or not the diagnosis was set independently, or whether any diagnostic instruments was used. Also, number of one-to-one teaching sessions provided by the parents was unspecified. This study also failed to employ a random assignment. The measures were not performed blind or independently, and did not include adaptive functioning. Children in the treatment group improved significantly more over a period of months than those in the control group. The remaining nine studies evaluated ABA treatments (Andersen, Avery, DiPietro, Edwards, & Christian, 1987; Birnbrauer & Leach, 1993; Eldevik et al., 2006; Lovaas, 1987; McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993; Magiati, Charman, & Howlin, 2007; Sallows & Graupner, 2005; Sheinkopf & Siegel, 1998; Smith, Buch, & Gamby, 2000; Weiss, 1999). The Lovaas (1987) and McEachin et al. (1993) studies received Level 3 scientific merit because intake measures did not include adaptive functioning.
Insufficient scientific value:
Nine outcome studies were classified as having insufficient scientific value. Six studies evaluated ABA programs (Bibby et al., 2002; Handelman, Harris, Celbiberti, Lilleheht, & Tomchek, 1991; Harris, Handleman, Gordon, Kristoff, & Fuentes, 1991, Harris, Handleman, Kristoff, Bass, & Gordon, 1990; Hoyson, Jamieson, Strain, 1984; Luiselli, Cannon, Ellis, Sisson, 2000), one evaluated TEACCH (Lord & Schopler, 1989), two evaluated the Colorado Health Science Program (Rogers & Dilalla, 1991; Rogers, Herbison, Lewis, Pantone, & Reiss, 1986). All studies used a pre-post design without a single-case control or comparison group.
Discussion:
Only one study received Level 1 scientific merit (the highest possible rating) and four studies received Level 2 scientific merits. All these studies evaluated ABA treatment. Eleven outcome studies received Level 3 rating. Nine of the 11 studies evaluated ABA treatments and 2 studies evaluated TEACCH. Finally, nine outcome studies were classified as having insufficient scientific value. One evaluated TEACCH, two evaluated the Colorado Health Science Program, and six evaluated ABA.
Evaluating magnitude of treatment effects, four ABA studies received Level 1 rating showing that children receiving ABA made significantly more gains than control group children on standardized measures of IQ, language and adaptive functioning (Cohen et al., 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2002, 2007; Howard et al., 2005; Sallows & Graupner, 2005). Several studies also included data on maladaptive behavior, personality, school performance and changes in diagnosis. Three studies received Level 2 rating (Eldevik et al., 2006; Lovaas, 1987; Smith, Groen, & Wynn, 2000), demonstrating that ABA treated children made significantly more gains than the comparison group on one standardized measures of IQ or adaptive functioning. Finally, five ABA studies and two TEACCH studies received Level 3 rating.
Based on these guidelines interventions based on ABA will be considered ‘‘well established’’. TEACCH and Colorado Health Science model will be considered neither ‘‘well established’’, nor ‘‘probably efficacious’’.
Future Direction:
- There is need for additional outcome studies, particularly those whose study designs are of level 1 standard.
- Effective treatment parameters and mechanisms responsible for change need to be identified and should be priority for ABA researchers.
- Variables that interact with or have an impact on outcomes should be identified. Treatment for children who respond less favorably needs to be established.
- Further research evaluating the efficacy of bio-medical treatments combined with psycho-social treatment is required.
- Research could examine the generalization and transportability of interventions shown to be efficacious in controlled research settings to applied settings.
- Research could be conducted to examine the efficacy of psycho-educational treatments with older children and adults.
- Research could develop criteria for discontinuing or changing treatment approach.
- Research could be conducted to examine the cost-effectiveness and cost-benefits of the interventions.
Conclusion:
- ABA treatment is demonstrated effective in enhancing global functioning in pre- school children with autism when treatment is intensive and carried out by trained therapists.
- ABA treatment is demonstrated effective in enhancing global functioning in children with PDD-NOS.
- ABA can be effective for children who are up to 7 years-of-age at intake.
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
Download the original study as PDF
For the summary and the translation a heartfelt thank you to Caroline Diziol.
Thanks a lot for allowing this study to be summarized, translated and published to: Kathy Hill, business manager of JABA
Great thanks for the permission to post and translate to: Svein Eikeseth, Ph. D. Professor, NOVA Institute for Children with Developmental Disorders, www.novaautism.com, Phone: (+47) 33 61 42 97, (+47) 92 21 09 88, www.novaautism.com.
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
10. Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism
Summary of the study
(PHILIP TEITELBAUM*, OSNAT TEITELBAUM*, JENNIFER NYE*, JOSHUA FRYMAN*, AND RALPH G. MAURER)
Introduction:
The idea that movement disorders play a role in autism has been debated. For example Rimland has argued the majority of autistic individuals are relatively unimpaired with regard to their gross motor skills capabilities and finger dexterity. Contrary autistic individuals can be found to be particularly coordinated and dexterous. This has been especially noted in autistic children who build tower blocks higher than normal adults and can climb to extreme heights without falling. According to Rimland this dismisses the notion that autism is or involves a movement disorder.
However, Damasio, Maurer and Vilensky et al, found that autistic children aged between 3–10 walked more slowly with shorter steps than normal and walked like adults who suffered from Parkinson. In addition, Courchesne et al, found that certain areas of the cerebellar vermis are not fully developed in autistic children. This supports the view that movement disorders may play a role in autism.
The aim of the current study is to try to resolve the issue of whether movement disorder plays a role in autism or not. As movement disorders can be detected as early as the first few days after birth, a study investigating movement disorders in infancy may serve as an early indicator for diagnosing autism in children.
Method:
Parents of children with autism (diagnosed via conventional methods before age three) were asked to send in videos of their children when they were infants. 17 infants were compared in their patterns of lying, righting from their back to their stomach, sitting, crawling, standing, and walking with 15 normal infants. Selected portions of these behaviors were transferred to rewriteable software for still analysis. The normal infants were filmed by the researchers at a stage when each pattern was just beginning.
Results:
Lying:
Lying is an active posture displayed by all newborn babies from the first few days of life. Constant digression from normal patterns of lying can indicate abnormalities associated with autism. For example, one of the children when lying on his stomach always had his right arm caught under his chest. This was persistent through the first year of his life, causing him to fall to his right side when lying on his stomach, sitting, and even when he started to walk.
An autistic child, ‘3 months old, lacking the ability to rotate around the body midline during righting a), attempts to sit up by ventroflexing his body in the midline plane (b).
Righting from spine to prone:
This is the ability to roll over from your back to stomach. This movement typically begins at age 3 months. The researchers of this study have found that in their experience impairments in righting are common in autistic children. From the 17 videos of autistic children that were analysed for this study only 3 of them had filmed the righting of autistic children. However the pattern of righting conveyed by these 3 infants was different from form shown by normal children. This abnormal pattern of righting was noted in the autistic children from age 3 months.
An autistic baby, 5 months old, cannot right by rotation. Instead, he arches the head and pelvis sideways upward, moves the top leg forward, and topples over en bloc, without the sequential segmental rotation in the righting movement characteristic of normal children.
Sitting:
From about age 6 months babies can sit in an upright position. Typically, autistic children are unable to maintain a stable sitting position. This study found that because an autistic child had an inability to distribute his/her weight equally on both sides the child falls over when reaching for objects.
An autistic girl, 8.5 months old, shows no allied protective reflexes when falling (e.g., extending the arms and hands out to protect herself from striking her head when falling toward the ground).
Crawling on hands and knees:
Most babies begin to crawl about the same time they begin to sit. There are different types of crawling including creeping and crawling. This study examined crawling on hands and knees. When crawling forward on hands and knees, the arms and thighs move parallel to the midline axis of the body. That means that the arms stay shoulder width apart, and so do the thighs.
A normal baby, 6 months old, shows good support in the arms and legs while crawling forward.
Some autistic children show digression from the normal patterns on crawling. One of the infants (3 months old) examined for this study supported himself on his forearms rather than his hands. With this particular child one arm was crossed in front of the other meaning that his base of support on his arms are very narrow, therefore making the right arm weaker than the left. Reaching was done with the left arm as the right arm was caught under the body. At age 6 months the child’s arms had developed support so the kegs could be used for crawling. However, the child exhibited a right side deficiency in the use of his legs for crawling; the left leg moved in the usual way, but the right leg did not move actively. This pattern was also noted in another autistic child in the videos.
An autistic baby, 5 months old, is unable to support himself on his hands and is unable to bring his knees toward his chest to crawl forward, so he lifts his rump up while trying to crawl but cannot move forward from the spot.
Standing:
Typical developing infants begin to pull them self up to stand for a few minutes at about 8–10 months old. One autistic girl in the video about 8–10 months old was seen standing in one place leaning against a piece of furniture for periods as long as 15 minutes. Such relative akinesia may signal abnormality.
Typical child standing at 10 months: holds his arms up at shoulder level as he is just beginning to learn to walk.
Walking:
When a baby starts to walk, his walking pattern develops through fixed stages which is controlled by different segments of the leg, with more control from the hip and the pelvis. The thigh is the first to make active movements. There are three stages involved when a child begins to walk; waddling, intermediate stage and final stage. These three stages can be observed in all children when they begin to walk. However, the amount of time children spend in each stage varies greatly from a few days to a few weeks. The walking pattern of autistic children differs from that of typical developing children. For example in typical developing children the arms and legs are symmetrical, in the autistic children these movements are asymmetrical.
At the age of two or older the walking patterns of autistic children are delayed in comparison to their typically developing peers. At the age of five abnormal patterns of walking could still be noted in one of the children in the video. In typical developing children the shift of weight usually occurs at the same time that the thigh and lower leg and the foot actively move forward. However, in the image of the autistic child this shift in weight occurred after the active movement forward of the thigh, lower leg and foot.
The position of the arm can serve as an important milestone along the course of normal development. For example, in a study conducted by Vilensky, Damasio, and Maurer, several autistic children (ages 3–10) exhibited more infantile positions of the arms while walking: the forearm often was held parallel to the ground, pointing forward.
Arm and hand flapping can also be noted in autistic children. This can also be noted in typically developing children but it disappears after a few months. However, if this persists for a sustained period of time (2 years or more) the arm and hand flapping may be an indication of autism.
(a) A 5‑year-old autistic boy has a fully developed step gesture. All three segments of the leg move actively (see text), but his body weight does not shift at the same time, resulting in a form of goose-step. (b) The body weight only then is shifted so that the boy falls on to the outstretched leg at each step. This is a form of sequencing rather than superimposition of one movement on the other.
Discussion:
Autism is usually diagnosed at age three when particular social skills fail to develop in children. However, social skills are not evident in the infancy stage, as the child largely relates to himself at this early stage. Although, the mother is usually aware during the early stages that something may be wrong, she cannot specify behaviors that are socially relevant for a diagnosis to be provided. As research has shown that almost all autistic children at later stages have movement abnormalities, these researchers in this study reasoned that such movement abnormalities will be evident from early infancy. The findings in this paper highlight the importance of detecting abnormalities in an infant’s movements from the early stages of life. If, as noted in this study and other studies, that children with movement abnormalities go on to be diagnosed with autism, it may be crucial for the purpose of early intervention that abnormalities in movement are considered as an important indicator of autism. This study also noted that these abnormalities were typically noted to occur on the right side of the body. An awareness of the abnormal movements as noted in this study should be of particular importance to paediatricians who can fail to detect these early signs of autism.
This study is a summary of the original paper (Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 13982–13987, November 1998, Psychology)
Download the original study as PDF
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
We received permission to summarize, to post and to translate this study from PNF.
Copyright: PNAS permissions 200826
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
11. Study about Early Diagnosis
Summary of the study
Half of children with autism can be accurately diagnosed at close to one year of age, new study shows
In a study published in the Archives of General Psychiatry, researchers from the Kennedy Krieger Institute in Baltimore, Maryland found that autism can be diagnosed at close to one year of age, which is the earliest the disorder has ever been diagnosed. This is the earliest time this disorder has ever been diagnosed. The study, which evaluated social and communication development in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) from 14 to 36 months of age, revealed that approximately half of all children with autism can be diagnosed around the first birthday. The remaining half will be diagnosed later, and their development may unfold very differently than children whose ASD is diagnosable around the first birthday. Early diagnosis of the disorder allows for early intervention, which can make a major difference in helping children with autism reach their full potential.
Researchers examined social and communication development in infants at high and low risk for ASD. Starting at 14 months of age and ending at 30 or 36 months (a small minority of the children exited the study at 30 months). Half of the children with a final diagnosis of ASD made at 30 or 36 months of age had been diagnosed with the disorder at 14 months, and the other half were diagnosed after 14 months. Through repeated observation and the use of standardized tests of development, researchers identified, for the first time, disruptions in social, communication and play development that were indicative of ASD in 14-month olds. Multiple signs indicating these developmental disruptions appear simultaneously in children with the disorder.
Dr. Rebecca Landa, lead study author and director of Kennedy Krieger’s Center for Autism and Related Disorders, and her colleagues identified the following signs of developmental disruptions for which parents and pediatricians should be watching:
Abnormalities in initiating communication with others: Rather than requesting help to open a jar of bubbles through gestures and vocalizations paired with eye contact, a child with ASD may struggle to open it themselves or fuss, often without looking at the nearby person.
Compromised ability to initiate and respond to opportunities to share experiences with others: Children with ASD infrequently monitor other people’s focus of attention. Therefore, a child with ASD will miss cues that are important for shared engagement with others, and miss opportunities for learning as well as for initiating communication about a shared topic of interest. For example, if a parent looks at a stuffed animal across the room, the child with ASD often does not follow the gaze and also look at the stuffed animal. Nor does this child often initiate communication with others. In contrast, children with typical development would observe the parent’s shift in gaze, look at the same object, and share in an exchange with the parent about the object of mutual focus. During engagement, children have many prolonged opportunities to learn new words and new ways to play with toys while having an emotionally satisfying experience with their parent.
Irregularities when playing with toys: Instead of using a toy as it is meant to be used, such as picking up a toy fork and pretending to eat with it, children with ASD may repeatedly pick the fork up and drop it down, tap it on the table, or perform another unusual act with the toy.
Significantly reduced variety of sounds, words and gestures used to communicate: Compared to typically developing children, children with ASD have a much smaller inventory of sounds, words and gestures that they use to communicate with others.
“For a toddler with autism, only a limited set of circumstances — like when they see a favorite toy, or when they are tossed in the air — will lead to fleeting social engagement,” said Landa. “The fact that we can identify this at such a young age is extremely exciting, because it gives us an opportunity to diagnose children with ASD very early on when intervention may have a great impact on development.”
The current study reveals that autism often involves a progression, with the disorder claiming or presenting itself between 14 and 24 months of age. Some children with only mild delays at 14 months of age could go on to be diagnosed with ASD. Landa and her colleagues observed distinct differences in the developmental paths, or trajectories, of children with early versus later diagnosis of ASD. While some children developed very slowly and displayed social and communication abnormalities associated with ASD at 14 months of age, others showed only mild delays with a gradual onset of autism symptoms, culminating in the diagnosis of ASD by 36 months.
If parents suspect something is wrong with their child’s development, or that their child is losing skills during their first few years of life, they should talk to their pediatrician or another developmental expert. This and other autism studies suggest that the “wait and see” method, which is often recommended to concerned parents, could lead to missed opportunities for early intervention during this time period.
“What’s most exciting about these important advancements in autism diagnosis is that ongoing intervention research leads us to believe it is most effective and least costly when provided to younger children,” said Dr. Gary Goldstein, President and CEO of the Kennedy Krieger Institute. “When a child goes undiagnosed until five or six years old, there is a tremendous loss of potential for intervention that can make a marked difference in that child’s outcome.”
While there are currently no standardized, published criteria for diagnosing children with autism at or around one year of age, Landa’s goal is to develop these criteria based on this and other autism studies currently underway at the Kennedy Krieger Institute. Landa and her colleagues at the Institute plan on releasing preliminary diagnostic criteria for very young children with autism in an upcoming report.
Participants in the current study included infants at high risk for ASD (siblings of children with autism, n=107) and low risk for ASD (no family history of autism, n=18). Standardized tests of development and play-based assessment tools were used to evaluate social interaction, communication and play behaviors in both groups at 14, 18 and 24 months of age. Researchers assigned diagnostic impressions at every age, indicating whether there were clinically significant signs of delay or impairment. After their last evaluation at 30 or 36 months, each participant was then given a final diagnostic classification of ASD, non-ASD impairment, or no impairment. The ASD group was further divided into an Early ASD diagnosis group and a Later ASD diagnosis group based on whether they were given a diagnosis of ASD at 14 or 24 months.
About Autism :
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is the nation’s fastest growing developmental disorder, with current incidence rates estimated at 1 in 150 children. This year more children will be diagnosed with autism than AIDS, diabetes and cancer combined. Yet profound gaps remain in our understanding of both the causes and cures of the disorder. Continued research and education about developmental disruptions in individuals with ASD is crucial, as early detection and intervention can lead to improved outcomes in individuals with ASD.
Source: Kennedy Krieger Institute
Great thanks for the permission to post and translate to: Dr. Rebecca Lang, Kennedy Krieger Institute .
Download summary of the study as PDF
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
12. A distinction between two behavior analytic approaches
Summary of the study
A summarization of the study BAT:
Behavioral Intervention for Autism: A distinction between two behavior analytic approaches
(Kelly Kates-McElrath and Saul Axelrod — Temple University)
Purpose of the Article:
Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) has come to be accepted as the treatment of choice for children with autism by professionals and parents alike (Schreibman, 1997). With this acceptance comes an increasing demanding for programs that employ the ABA methodology to be implemented for pre-school and school-aged children diagnosed with ASD in school settings. Therefore, it is vital for school personnel to understand the distinction between different types of programs that fall under the umbrella of ABA and what is implied when parents request discrete trial or applied verbal behavior programs.
Gresham, Beebe-Frankenberger and MacMillan (1999) evaluated a number of programs behavioral and educational treatment programs for children with autism. These included, the UCLA Young Autism Project (YAP), based on the work by O. Ivar Lovaas (1987); Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children (Project TEACCH), based on the work of Schopler and Reichler (1971); and Learning Experiences Alternative Program (LEAP), based on the work of Strain and others (1977). Since this evaluation, other program that are behavioral analytical in nature have been employed. These include Pivotal Response Training (PRT) (Koegel, Koegel, & Carter, 1999) and Applied Verbal Behavior (AVB) (Sundberg & Michael, 2001).
Due to the number of intervention programs under the umbrella of ABA, the purpose of this paper is to distinguish between two popular approaches currently provided for early intervention and school-aged children in home- and school-based settings: Lovaas’ Young Autism Project (YAP), more commonly referred to as Discrete Trial Instruction (DTI) or Discrete Trial Teaching (DTT), and B.F. Skinner’s Analysis of Verbal Behavior, more commonly referred to as Applied Verbal Behavior (AVB or simply VB). This paper refers to DTT as DTI.
Background and results from Lovaas:
All participants in the original Lovaas study (1987) had a diagnosis of autism and a chronological age of less than 40 months if non-verbal, and less than 46 months if presented with echolalia. The experimental group (n=19) received intensive one-to-one treatment for more than 40 hours per week for two years, whereas Control Group 1 (n=19) received minimal one-to-one treatment, characterized by 10 hours or less, also for two years. Participants were assigned to one of these two groups based on the number of available staff and the distance participants lived from UCLA. An additional control group (Control Group 2) was comprised of 21 participants selected from those participating in a previous study by Freeman, Ritvo, Needleman, & Yokota (1985). Data from this control group helped to control for biased participant selection. Participants were treated like Control Group 1 subjects but were not treated by the DTI team. The goal of this project was to maximize treatment gains by providing the intervention for most of the participants’ waking hours. Results showed 47% of the participants in the experimental treatment group achieved normal intellectual functioning as defined by normal-range IQ scores and successful performance in first grade in a public school setting (Lovaas, 1987).
DTI, as implemented in the original Lovaas study (1987), is a specialized form of instruction that breaks down tasks/instructions into smaller teachable units. This consists of a cue (SD), prompt, student response, and a consequence (i.e., reinforcement or feedback in the form of error correction). Gresham et al. (1999) define the core characteristics of DTI as a Discriminative Stimulus (SD)-response-consequence type of instructional delivery that includes discrimination training and compliance with instructional commands (e.g., “Stand up” and “Touch your nose”).
Background to the AVB Approach:
The AVB approach to teaching children with autism incorporates discrete trial instruction; however, for language acquisition it relies on B.F. Skinner’s classification of language with initial emphasis on teaching expressive language with manding (Carbone, 2003; Carbone, 2004; Sundberg & Partington, 1998). Although this approach has not been promoted by professionals as an educational treatment package or method, consumers of this approach have taken it as such.
This approach emphasizes the formal and functional properties of language and distinguishes between several different types of functional control (Sundberg, 2003). Skinner defined the mand as a type of verbal relation whose response form is controlled by a motivational variable, termed establishing operation (EO) (i.e. satiation, deprivation, and aversive stimulation), or more recently termed, motivational operation (MO) (Laraway, Snycerski, Michael, & Poling, 2003). The mand is a type of verbal behavior where the speaker asks for what he or she wants, resulting in specific reinforcement (i.e., access to a desired item specific to the request) (Sundberg, 2003). Other verbal relations proposed by Skinner are tacts (labelling items in the environments), echoic (repeating what is said), intraverbal (responding to a verbal stimulus), textual (reading) transcriptive (writing).
Advocates of the AVB approach credit Lovaas and colleagues for their contribution and advancement to the field of ABA in autism treatment but criticize their work for failing to implement the concepts and principles provided by Skinner in his book Verbal Behavior (1957). Particularly, Lovaas and colleagues’ failure to make use of early mand training and transfer control procedures to teach across all the verbal operants.
Differences in the Curriculum DTI and AVB approaches:
The curriculum scope and sequence for DTI programs is derived from resources such as Teaching Developmentally Disabled Children, The Me Book (Lovaas et al., 1981), Behavioral Intervention for Young Children with Autism (Maurice, Green, & Luce, 1996), A Work in Progress (Leaf & McEachin, 1999), and more recently, Teaching Individuals with Developmental Delays, Basic Intervention Techniques (Lovaas, 2003). Considering, that there is no standard assessment practice and numerous curriculum resources, each child’s program varies with regard to the order in which new tasks are presented. Skills generally begin being taught in the simplest format and increasing in complexity. Generalising each skill involves the children practising the skill across instructors, materials, and settings, as well as programming for common stimuli and using multiple exemplars.
AVB programs rely on the Assessment of Basic Language and Learning Skills (ABLLS) (Partington & Sundberg, 1998) as a standard assessment tool and baseline. The completed ABLLS provides a visual display of the learner’s strengths and weaknesses across 26 skill domains. No other guides to curriculum or teaching targeting this approach are commercially available.
Differences in Reinforcement and Motivation in DTI and AVB approaches:
DTI programs typically employ a negative reinforcement paradigm for learner motivation (i.e., the student can work for earned breaks from task) (Harris & Weiss, 1998). Additionally, other components of an individualized motivational system such as token systems of reinforcement and choice boards comprised of photos of potential rewards are incorporated.
The AVB approach places emphasis on the teacher initially becoming a conditioner reinforcer for the child. This is gained through pairing the teacher with reinforcement and demand fading procedure. The AVB approach focuses on the issue of positive reinforcement and motivation to increase on-task behaviors. Dense schedules of reinforcement for initial mand training are continuous; fading to thinner and/or variable schedules are implemented as quickly as possible during intensive teaching time (ITT) and faded as the learner is successful (Carbone, 2004). In an AVB program, there tends to be less reliance on token boards, choice boards, and other visual displays that are common to motivational programs in a DTI approach.
Differences in the Delivery of instruction in DTI and AVB approaches:
In DTI programs, instruction is typically delivered via a 1:1 or 1:2 teacher-to-student ratios (Harris & Weiss, 1998). The teacher and student are usually situated at a desk or table facing one another. Instruction is introduced in an environment where distractions are minimized. Novel concepts are often introduced in isolation or mass trials.
AVB programs also employ the 1:1 or 1:2 teacher-to student ratio, however, the initial phases of teaching take place in the natural environment and not at the table. AVB programs emphasises Natural Environment Teaching (NET) also referred to as incidental teaching. This form of teaching relies on the student’s motivation for instruction and there is no specified teaching place. The delivery of instruction during ITT is the same as that of discrete trial instruction. Both approaches would suggest a ratio of easy-to-hard tasks that is approximately 8:2 or 7:3. In addition, the AVB approach emphasizes teaching skills to fluency and a quick pace of instruction with shorter latencies for the learner to respond (0–2 seconds as opposed to a traditional DTI approach of 5–7 seconds).
Differences in prompting and Error Correction Procedure in DTI and AVB approaches:
In DTI programs, initial reliance is on errorless teaching procedures such as a most-to least prompting sequence, constant and progressive time delay, stimulus fading, positional cues, and blocked errors (Agnew & Kates-McElrath, 2004). As the learner acquires skills, the no-no-prompt error correction procedure is introduced. This procedure presumes the student can respond correctly to the instruction or self-correct following a “No” or no alternative (“Try again”) from the teacher. This approach allows for two errors before prompting is provided (Pelios & Kates-McElrath, 2002).
Although both rely on errorless teaching methods as described above, the AVB approach does not employ the no-no-prompt model of error correction. In addition, it places added emphasis on transfer trials following errors of responding (Carbone, 2003).
Differences in Language acquisition in DTI and AVB approaches:
Traditional DTI programs place an earlier emphasis on receptive identification and/or expressive labelling (tacting) of objects or photos rather, than teaching students to request desired items (manding) as in AVB programs. In DTI listener skills are targeted before speaker skills. As stated earlier the initial stages of the AVB program involves stimulus-stimulus paring. During this stage the child’s naturally occurring vocalization(s) (i.e., babbling sounds) is established as a conditioned reinforcer through the temporal pairing of a therapist’s vocal model with a desired item.
Differences in Data Collection Procedures in DTI and AVB approaches:
Traditional DTI programs rely on teachers and therapists to collect trial-by-trial data that reflect student performances during teaching (Harris & Weiss, 1998), often yielding a percent correct in 10 or 20 trials. Task analytic data are collected on skills targeting leisure, self-care, and vocational domains.
AVB programs are characterized by first trial yes / no probe data. Probe data are often ideally collected in the morning (school-based probe) and evening (home-based probe) for evidence of generalisation across settings and materials. Probe data allows the teacher to be available to focus on teaching as opposed to recording each student response. It also facilitates a quicker pace of instruction (Carbone, 2003).
Both approaches rely on visual displays of data as well as data-based decisions regarding student progress and program changes (Harris & Weiss, 1998).
The AVB approach however, favors cumulative graphing over traditional percentages or number correct (Carbone, 2003).
Recommendations for future research:
Future research should involve outcome measures for both approaches. Comparative research between both approaches is also needed to assess which if any of the approaches is better than the other at increasing language acquisition in children with autism.
For a more comprehensive read and further information please download the paper from Behavior Analyst Today, VOLUME 7, Issue 2, p.242: http://www.behavior-analyst-today.net
For the permission to post and translate this study from BAT great thanks to: Dr. Joe Cautilli.
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
Download summary of the study as PDF
o
13. Study about early Intensive Behavioral Intervention
Summary of the study
A summarization of the study from AJMR:
Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention: Outcomes for Children With Autism and Their Parents After Two Years
(Bob Remington, University of Southampton, UK; Richard P. Hastings, University of Wales, Bangor, UK; Hanna Kovshoff and Francesca degli Espinosa, University of Southampton, UK; Erik Jahr, Akershus University Hospital, Norway; Tony Brown, Paula Alsford, Monika Lemaic, and Nicholas Ward, University of Southampton, UK)
Background:
This study assessed the effects of Early Intensive Behavior Interventions (EIBI) for preschool children with autism in Southern England. EIBI is a highly structured and intensive teaching approach based on the principles of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA). Children are taught a range of skills by trained therapists. They break down skills into small teachable units that are easily accessible for the learners. Prior to this study there has been strong research evidence suggesting that EIBI is effective for a wide variety of children with autism. However the majority of this evidence is based on data from America. And prior to these findings there had been no data from a UK sample to support the efficacy of EIBI for use with preschool children with autism in the UK.
The purpose of the research:
The authors of this research designed this study to address three key questions:
- Can EIBI reduce the diagnostics symptoms associated with autism?
- Can EIBI have a positive impact on the language, cognitive and behavioral deficits associated with autism?
- Does EIBI contribute to increased family pressures?
Method:
Two groups of preschool children diagnosed with autism were recruited for this study. The first group of children consisted of 23 preschool children diagnosed with autism receiving EIBI for a period of two years. The second group consisted of 21 children diagnosed with autism receiving a standard educational provision from their local education authority for a period of two years. Assessments for both groups took place before, a year into, at the end, and two years after the research began.
Participants:
All children had received a diagnosis of autism and presented with no other medical or chronic illness. Children in both groups were aged between 30 months and 42 months and all lived in their family home.
Measures:
A range of standardized tests was used to assess the children, including IQ test. Parents were also assessed for their psychological well-being.
Procedure:
Children in the EIBI group received one-to-one intervention in their homes for an average of 25 hours per week over a two year period. The intervention was delivered by 3–5 tutors from a range of service providers and also by the parents of the children, who were all trained to teach using the principles of ABA. The EIBI intervention group was taught a variety of key skills which included, play, language and cognition and adaptive behaviors.
The children in the group not receiving EIBI, received standard provision from their local education authority and some form of speech and language therapy.
Results:
Results found 26% of the children receiving EIBI demonstrated substantial gains in IQ. Results further illustrated that there were significant improvements in intelligence, daily living skills, motor skills, social skills, and in early social communication and language. Relative to the parents whose children received standard provision, no increase in psychological adjustment problems were noted with the parents whose children received EIBI. Die Results also found a decrease in the problem behaviors and diagnostic symptoms associated with autism. Differences between the two groups were still noted after 12 months.
Conclusion:
The findings from this study demonstrated that EIBI can be effectively implemented in the UK. Although the intervention fell short of the recommended 40 hours per week, results were on the whole comparable to those provided by the US. Although the key questions of the research had been addressed, the findings also brought additional questions. For example, it remains unclear which children are most likely to benefit from EIBI, how to best evaluate and identify effective teaching methodologies and curricula, the long term effects of EIBI and whether EIBI can provide better outcomes than those reported thus far.
This study is a summary of the original documents. For a more comprehensive read and further information please contact Bob Remington to receive the full study as PDF at: R.E.Remington@soton.ac.uk
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
Download summary of the study as PDF
We received permission to summarize, to post and to translate this study from AJMR.
Order Detail ID: 19908526
AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MENTAL RETARDATION by R.E. Remington. Copyright 2007 by American Association on Intellectual Developmental Disabilities. Reproduced with permission of American Association on Intellectual Developmental Disabilities in the format Internet posting via Copyright Clearance Center.
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
14. The Role of the Reflexive Conditioned Motivating Operation (CMO‑R) During Discrete Trial Instruction of Children with Autism
Summary of the study
A summarization of the study from JEIBI:
The Role of the Reflexive Conditioned Motivating Operation (CMO‑R) During Discrete Trial Instruction of Children with Autism
(Vincent J. Carbone, Barry Morgenstern, Gina Zecchin-Tirri & Laura Kolberg)
Background:
There has been strong scientific evidence to indicate that the implementation of the principles (Reinforcement, Extinction, Punishment, Stimulus Control and Motivating Operations) Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) is an effective intervention for children with autism over that of other interventions.
Results from such research has demonstrated that children who are taught intensively (25–40 hours per week) following the principles of behavior analysis (as listed above) can make substantial gains in cognitive abilities and developing age appropriate social skills, Lovaas (1987).
The purpose of the research:
Much of the research into the application of ABA for children with autism has emphasized the importance motivating these children to comply with and respond to teacher directed instructional tasks. According to Koegel, Carter and Koegel (1998) motivation is pivotal to the teaching of children with autism; its creation can lead to the development of a wide range of skills.
A fundamental component of intensive ABA programs for children with autism is the implementation of discrete trial instruction. Discrete trial instruction follows the three-term-contingency arrangement as proposed by Skinner (1968). This involves: the presentation of a stimulus by an instructor, the occurrence of the response, and a consequence which follows the response, in order to strengthen or weaken the likelihood of that response occurring under similar conditions. Although discrete trial instruction is highly beneficial in the acquisition of skills, the high demand requirements of this method are the same conditions that typically evoke problem behavior in the form of tantrumming, flopping, high rates of sterotypies, aggression, and self-injury.
Consequently, a thorough conceptual understanding and practical repertoire related to the modification of instructional variables that reduce escape and avoidance maintained problem behavior of children with autism appears essential. The purpose of this paper is to provide an overview of the behavioral analysis of motivation during discrete trial instruction and a re-interpretation of the effects of antecedent variables as motivation operations (MO), and more specifically, the reflexive motivating operation or CMO‑R.
The Establishing Operation
The term Establishing Operation (EO) as defined by Michael (1993) describes an environmental event or stimulus condition that makes someone “want something” and leads to actions that can produce to what is wanted. A large amount of problem behaviors (as described earlier) in children with autism during discrete trial instruction may result from a motivation of something (EO), for example, attention, toy, removal of tasks and demands. An EO that increases the value of a conditioned negative reinforcement and evokes any behavior that has led to a decrease in the present aversive condition is known as a Reflexive Conditioned Motivating Operation or CMO‑R.
The CMO‑R and Teaching Children with Autism
Responding maintained by escape and avoidance of instructional demands accounts for up to 48% of self-injurious and aggressive behaviors of persons with developmental disabilities (Derby et al., 1992; Iwata et al., 1994). These types of escape and avoidance behaviors interfere with learning. This is further complicated when instructions and demands during discrete trial instructions act as a CMO‑R (Sundberg, 1993).
Methods to Reduce the Effects of the CMO‑R During Discrete Trial Instruction:
1. Programming Competing Reinforcers
Behaviors maintained by negative reinforcement (e.g. the removal of a demand or task to engage in a preferred activity) can be weakened by programming differential reinforcement of alternative behaviors (DRA) or delivering reinforcement non-contingently (via NCR procedures) during high demand situations. Studies investigating participants whose problem behaviors had been acquired and maintained by negative reinforcement. They found that by programming concurrent schedules of reinforcement in which task demands were positively reinforced could lead to a decrease in problem behaviors without modifying maintaining contingencies or the use of extinction for problem behaviors. A study by DeLeon et al; (2002) investigated the effects of positive and negative reinforcement on problem behaviors maintained by negative reinforcement with a chained demand. A child with autism was provided the opportunity to choose a positive reinforcer (i.e., potato chip) or negative reinforcer (i.e., break) after completing a scheduled number of responses. When the number of demands was relatively low, the participant reliably chose the positive reinforcer. It appeared that the presence of the positive reinforcer decreased the value of task termination as a reinforcer. However, her preference switched to the break when the number of tasks required for reinforcement increased to more than 10. The authors concluded that the switch to the preference for a break when demands were increased indicated the demands had returned to their initial status as a CMO‑R and therefore increased the value of task removal and evoked the participant’s choice behavior of a break.
2. Pairing and Embedding the Instructional Environment with Positive Reinforcement
McGill (1999) suggests paring and embedding the teaching context, materials and personnel with an “improving set of conditions” via the delivery of positive reinforcement. This would reduce the averseness of the teaching environment, thus making escape and avoidance behaviors (often associated with problem behaviors) less likely.
3. Errorless Instruction
Research has illustrated that when students make frequent errors during an instructional task, problem behaviors often occur at a high rate. Instructional methods that reduce the frequency of errors have been demonstrated to reduce the level of problem behavior. “An analysis of these results in terms of motivational variables suggests that errors may function as an MO and increase the reinforcing value of task removal or termination. If the instructor prevents or at least minimizes errors during instruction (i.e., errorless learning) the CMO‑R is abolished and students engage in fewer problem behaviors.” Errorless learning has been employed via the use of response prompts, antecedent prompts. “The reduction in errors probably functioned as an abolishing operation that reduced the effectiveness of escape as a reinforcing consequence and as a result reduced escape-motivated problem behavior.”
4. Stimulus Demand Fading
Instructional demands are often associated with the CMO‑R in a number of studies. Such findings have demonstrated that escape motivated problem behaviors can be dramatically reduced by removing demands. However, such an approach would also significantly reduce the number of learning opportunities. Several studies have highlighted that demand fading wherein the instructor delivers one instructional demand at about the midpoint of the session. Over successive sessions, more demands were faded into the session. The results suggested that the fading procedures accelerated the behavior reduction effects of extinction. These results were probably obtained because the original task demands functioned as a CMO‑R that increased the value of escape-motivated problem behavior. Removal of demands weakened the MO and decreased escape-motivated problem behaviors. Their gradual re-introduction in some cases did not create enough of a CMO‑R to increase escape motivated problem behaviors. Modifying the rate, difficulty, and effort of responses during discrete trial instruction appears to reduce escape- and avoidance-motivated problem behaviors. Over time, instructors may be able to fade in the rate, difficulty, and effort of demands until high levels of instructional participation are reached without problem behavior.
5. Pace of Instruction
Studies have illustrated that short Inter-Trial-Intervals (ITI) are correlated with reduced stereotypic behaviors and higher correct rates of responding when compared to long ITI. Fast paced instruction has been correlated with less off-task behaviors and higher skill acquisition. “Pace of instruction probably functions as an abolishing operation, reducing the value of escape and avoidance as reinforcers. Specifically, during the ITI, reinforcement is not available and with longer, as compared to shorter intervals, the child receives a lower rate of reinforcement for instructional sessions of equal duration. A recent study by Roxburgh and Carbone (2007) investigated this issue directly and found that during instruction of children with autism, shorter ITIs produced a higher rate of reinforcement and therefore less problem behavior. During long ITIs, the learner likely receives automatic reinforcement for stereotypic behavior. In contrast, instructional demands delivered at a brisk pace reduce the rate of reinforcement available through automatic reinforcement and increases the rate of socially mediated positive reinforcement available.”
6. Interspersal Instruction
A number of studies have illustrated that problem behaviors can be decreased by interspersing easy and difficult tasks. Problem behaviors can be reduced during this procedure as the interspersal of “easy tasks functions as a CMO‑R because they are correlated with a worsening set of conditions related to low rates of reinforcement, high rates of errors, and higher rates of social disapproval. By interspersing easy tasks with more difficult tasks the value of the CMO‑R is reduced. It is recommended to combine extinction with interspersal instruction to ensure its effectiveness (Zarcone, Iwata, Hughes, & Vollmer, 1993). It is also important to avoid presenting easy tasks immediately following problem behavior. If this were to occur, problem behavior would likely be strengthened by negative reinforcement.”
Conclusion:
“A thorough understanding of the principle of motivation and an analysis of instructional methods as MOs can provide behavior analysts with a powerful technology for reducing problem behavior during discrete trial instruction. With knowledge of the concept of the CMO‑R, behavior analysts may be better equipped to evaluate, select, and implement instructional methods that reduce escape and avoidance behavior exhibited by a large percentage of children with autism and related disabilities.”
We hope that this has served as a useful introduction and summary into the concept of the CMO‑R.
Download summary of the study as PDF
For a more comprehensive read and further information please download the paper here.
For the permission to post this study from JEIBI great thanks to: Dr. Joe Cautilli.
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
15. Study about Manding
Summary of the study
A summarization of the study from the Carbone Clinic:
Increasing Vocalizations of Children with Autism Using Sign Language and Mand Training
(Vivian Attanasio, Lisa Delaney, Vincent J. Carbone, Gina Zecchin-Tirri, and Emily J. Sweeney-Kerwin)
Background:
Manual sign language has been shown to support the development of vocal verbal behavior in some individuals with autism and developmental disabilities. (Mirenda & Erickson, 2000; Mirenda, 2003; Tincani, 2004). However there is a subset of children with autism for whom sign language may not facilitate vocal production (Mirenda, 2003). In those cases it may be necessary to add other behavioral interventions to increase the development of vocal responding. Language training programs that manipulate motivative variables to teach manding have been shown to increase spontaneity (Shafer, 1994) and vocalizations (Charlop-Christy, Carpenter, LeBlanc & Kellett, 2002).
Skinner (1957) defined the mand as a verbal response which is evoked by some conditions of deprivation, satiation, or aversion and which is reinforced by a consequence specific to the motivational variable.
A time delay or prompt delay procedure following the presentation of a vocal model to increase vocal spontaneity and production, has been shown to be effective. (Halle, Marshall, & Spradlin1979; Halle, Baer, & Spradlin, 1981; Carr & Kologinsky, 1983; Charlop, Schreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985; Bennett, Gast, Wolery, & Schuster, 1986; Matson, Sevin, Frideley, & Love, 1990; Ingenmey & Van Houten, 1991; Charlop & Trasowech,1991; Matson, Sevin, Box, & Francis, 1993).
The time delay procedures implemented in these studies had been shown to be effective with participants who had a vocal repertoire that was dependent upon prompting. The time delay procedure had not been previously tested for its value initiating new vocal responses in children with autism who used sign language as their primary form of communication instead of vocalizations, or had few vocalizations.
The purpose of the research:
The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of sign mand training combined with a time delay, vocal prompt and differential reinforcement procedure on the development of vocalizations in children with autism for whom sign language mand training alone had not produced vocal responding.
Method:
Participants:
Three male participants were used in this study. Two of the participants were age four (Tony) and six (Nick) and both diagnosed with autism. The third participant was age four, diagnosed with Down Syndrome (Ralph). All participants had limited receptive, tact and intraverbal repertoires.
Tony manded via American Sign Language (ASL) for 15 items, those were present and highly motivating. The Kaufman Assessment (Kaufman Speech Praxis Assessment, 1995) found that Tony had a weak echoic repertoire which involved approximations to (consonsant — vowel — consonant (CVC) (CVC words for example, “oh no” and “oboe.”).
Nick required partial physical prompts or full physical prompts to produce his manual sign mands. However his manding repertoire was considered as weak. During the modified Kaufman Speech Praxis Assessment, Nick did not echo any vocal responses.
Ralph used manual sign language to mand for 10 items that were present and highly motivating.
Setting:
The study was conducted in each of the participant’s classroom. Each room had six to eight children with at least three (3) adults.
Defining Responses:
This study measured the occurrence of vocalizations during sign manding, either following a time delay or after the presentation of a vocal prompt. Any sound made by the participant was identified and recorded as a vocalization. A word approximation was defined as a vowel-consonant (VC) or consonant-vowel (CV) combination that were in the name of the item presented.
Recording Procedure:
Baseline:
Vocalizations and approximations were recorded by the participant’s instructor. Instructors sat next to the child at a table with their data sheets. Six potential reinforcers, which included edibles, movies and toys, were placed in a random rotation throughout the session approximately one foot away from the learner. Instructors recorded vocalizations or word approximations by writing the phonetic spelling of each vocal response with the prompt level necessary to evoke the vocal response. Inter-observer agreement was conducted for 30% of all sessions. Inter-observer agreement ranged between 96%-100% with an average of 99% percent agreement.
Time Delay and Vocal Prompt:
During the intervention phase, the examiner sat at a table approximately two feet across from the participant in his classroom. The items the participant would potentially mand for were on the table next to the experimenter. All participants had six different reinforcers present at every session. There were two sessions per day each consisting of 50 trials. Each trial began with the experimenter holding the item at eye level as a signal to the participant that reinforcement was available contingent on them signing for the item. When the participant signed, the reinforcer was not immediately delivered and instead a five second time delay was implemented. During the five second delay, any vocalization by the participant resulted in delivery of the reinforcer immediately.
If the participant did not vocalize during the time delay interval, the experimenter would say the name of the desired item as a vocal prompt and wait two seconds for a response.
If a vocalization occurred within two seconds of the presentation of the vocal stimulus (prompt) the reinforcer was delivered immediately.
If no vocalization occurred, the vocal prompt was re-presented two more times.
The reinforcer was delivered immediately upon hearing any vocalization or word approximation from the participant following the vocal prompts.
If no vocalization or word approximation occurred the reinforcer was delivered at the end of the sequence of presentations of the vocal prompts.
Maintenance:
Maintenance data was collected ten months following the completion of the experimental condition. Four sessions of maintenance data were conducted during which each targeted item was presented once. Each session consisted of six trials and there was one session a day for four consecutive days. Each trial began with the experimenter holding up the item to the participant’s eye level in order to signal the availability of the reinforcer.
If the participant signed for the item correctly within five seconds of the presentation the item was delivered immediately.
If the participant did not sign for the item immediately or signed incorrectly, the experimenter provided a manual or gestural prompt to evoke the response. Using the same response definition as in the experimental condition, the experimenter recorded the occurrence of any vocalization or word approximation that the participant produced when he signed. Inter-observer agreement was 100% across all participants.
Results:
When the experimental condition was introduced Tony’s vocalizations or word approximations went from an average of about 20% during baseline to about 95% in treatment. Tony maintained an average of about 95% of vocalizations or word approximations while manding during maintenance sessions.
The frequency of vocalizations or word approximations for Ralph immediately increased to approximately 70% from a baseline percentage of near zero upon entering treatment and then stabilized with about 95% of trials containing vocal responses for the last few treatment sessions. 10 month follow-up data indicates that Ralph’s vocalizations during sign manding were maintained at a level substantially higher than baseline.
Nick’s vocalizations immediately increased to approximately 10% during treatment and steadily increased to 40% throughout treatments sessions as compared to a baseline percentage of near zero. The ten month follow-up data indicated that Nick’s vocalizations while sign manding were maintained at a level substantially higher than baseline.
Discussion:
The results of this study demonstrate that sign mand opportunities combined with a prompt delay procedure and vocal prompting with differential reinforcement for sound production can increase the frequency and variety of vocalizations in children with autism and other developmental disabilities.
Tony demonstrated the highest percentage of trials with vocalizations and word approximations and was the only participant to develop the production of the word “movie” as a mand to view a video. He entered the study with the strongest vocal repertoire in that he produced the greatest number and variety of sounds during the baseline sound inventory.
Maintenance data show that Tony and Ralph continued to vocalize at a high rate while sign manding despite no treatment for a ten month period. The fact that the time delay procedure produced a relatively higher rate of vocalizations as compared to the vocal prompt procedure implicates the role of extinction. It appears that failure to reinforce the sign mand immediately during treatment may have led to response variation in the form of vocal responses consistent with the side-effects frequently associated with extinction (Lerman and Iwata, 1996).
This article is a summary of the original paper which can be downloaded from www.carboneclinic.com/research.aspx
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in the paper on your child.
Download summary of the study as PDF
For the permission to post this study we thank: Dr. Vincent Carbone, Ed.D., BCBA
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
16. Study about Fluent Teaching
Summary of the study
A summarization of the study from the Carbone Clinic:
The Effects of Varying Teacher Presentation Rates on Responding during Discrete Trial Training
(Carole A. Roxborough, BCABA, Vincent J. Carbone, BCBA, and Gina Zecchin, BCABA)
Background:
A significant amount of children diagnosed with autism engage in high rates of escape and avoidance behaviors (Koegel, Koegel, Frea and Smith, 1995) during instructional sessions. Additionally, self-stimulatory behavior (such as rocking and hand flapping) in children with autism often interferes with acquiring new skills and conducting simple discrimination tasks (Covert and Koegel, 1972). When self-stimulatory behavior is reduced learning occurs at a higher rate (Covert and Koegel, 1972).
Therefore, one of the fundamental aims for many children with autism may partially depend on teachers manipulating instructional variables. Those lead to improved learner attention to teacher directed activities for reasonable periods of time each day. (Drash & Tudor, 1993).
Discrete Trial Training (DTT) is a method which is modelled after Skinner’s (1968) three term contingency arrangement. whereby a stimulus is presented by a teacher, a response is evoked, and a consequence follows the response in order to strengthen or weaken its likelihood of occurring again under similar conditions.
The implementation of DTT has yielded long term benefits for children with autism (Lovaas, 1987, Smith, 1999, McEachin, Smith & Lovaas, 1993). However, the high demand requirements of Discrete Trial Training may evoke problem behavior such as tantrumming, flopping, high rates of self stimulatory behaviors, aggression, and self injury. Smith (2001) explains “… children with autism may attempt to escape or avoid almost all teaching situations, as well as any requests that adults make of them” (p. 89).
Consequently, a thorough conceptual understanding and practical repertoire related to the modification of instructional variables that reduce escape and avoidance maintained problem behavior of children with autism appears essential.
Manipulation of instructional variables related to the consequence of behaviors such as reinforcement and extinction have been extensively studied in the behavior reduction literature. Recently, additional emphasis has been placed upon the manipulation of antecedent variables to reduce interfering behaviors when teaching persons with developmental disabilities and autism (Carbone, Morgenstern & Zecchin (2006).
Little research has focused on the effects of teacher rate of presentation of instructional demands as an antecedent variable. Only two studies that included autistic children have measured the effects of teacher rate of presentation of instructional demands. Both of these studies (Koegel, Dunlap, & Dyer, (1980) and Dunlap, Dyer & Koegel (1983)) manipulated the duration of inter trial intervals (ITI) resulting in either slow or fast pace presentation of instructional demands. ITI was defined as the duration of time between the delivery of the consequence for one behavior and the presentation of the next instructional stimulus or demand.
Koegel et al (1980) investigated the functional relationship between ITI duration and correct learner responding in children with autism. The researchers used both long durations which ranged from 4 seconds to 26 seconds and short durations which range from 1 to 4 seconds. Results demonstrated that shorter duration of ITIs produced a higher rate of correct responses and a decrease in self stimulatory behaviors.
Dunlap et al (1983) replicated Koegel et al’s (1980) study and then extended the findings by precisely measuring occurrences of self-stimulatory responses in their participants who were children with autism. Results from this study found that self-stimulatory responses decreased with shorter ITI and correct responding increased.
The effects of teacher presentation rates on other topographies and functions of problem behavior frequently emitted by children with autism during intensive teaching sessions has been investigated with various manipulations of ITI’s (download article for a full review).
The purpose of the research:
There were four aims of this study:
- To replicate the findings of other researchers regarding the effects of altering the pace of instructional demands on the occurrences of problem behavior and correct responding during instructional settings with children with autism.
- To examine the effects of teacher rate of presentation of instructional demands with children with autism who exhibited self-stimulatory behavior and responses that appeared to be maintained by a history of social reinforcement.
- To measure opportunities to respond and magnitude of reinforcement as a function of faster vs. slower rates of teacher presentation of demands.
- To measure three rates of presentation commonly recommended in instructional programs for children with autism.
Method:
Participants:
Two children with a diagnosis of autism receiving a combination of school and home based intervention using Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) with emphasis upon teaching communication skills using B.F. Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior were used in this study.
Both children’s program included one-on-one intensive teaching in the form of Discrete Trial Training interspersed with learning opportunities in the more naturalized environments in the home setting. A similar program was implemented for both children in the school setting for part of the instructional day.
Both participants exhibited high rates of disruptive behavior during instructional sessions and therefore were selected to participate in this study.
Setting:
All of the experimental sessions were carried out in the home of each participant. The instructional setting for each child was in the family living room where a television was available to display videos as a form of reinforcement. Each child was seated at an instructional table. A video camera was also set up on a tripod next to the instructional table for purposes of recording each session.
Dependent variables, Response Definitions and Measurement Procedures:
The dependent variables which were measured were:
- Frequency of problem behavior (self-stimulatory behavior; aggression / self injurious behavior, bolting from the instructional table) that interfered with instructional demands were exhibited
- Frequency of teacher presented instructional demands
- Magnitude or duration of reinforcement
- Percentage of correct responses.
Each of the dependent variables was measured following each experimental session by transcribing the responses from the video recording of the session. A data recording sheet developed specifically to measure frequency of problem behavior, frequency of instructional opportunities, frequency of responses per session, magnitude of video presentation as a form of reinforcement, and percentage of correct and incorrect responses was used.
Design:
Using an alternating treatment design teacher demands were presented at the rate of every second, every five seconds or every 10 seconds during experimental sessions. For example, a teacher might hold up a picture of an object and ask the learner “what is it?”. Instructional demands which were presented 1 second after the participants responded were referred to as the fast teacher presentation condition. Instructional demands which were presented 5 seconds after the participants responded were referred to as the medium teacher presentation condition. Instructional demands which were presented 10 seconds after the participants responded were referred to as the slow teacher presentation condition. A non baseline alternating treatments design between 1, 5 and 10 second was implemented randomly.
Procedure:
Two sessions were conducted a day each lasting 10 minutes. Throughout each session the instructor presented instructional demands either every one, five or ten second interval. Instructional techniques including error correction, prompting procedures, types of skills presented, number of demands before a reinforcer (schedule of reinforcement), interspersal of mastered and target skills and mixing of skill domains were held constant for each participant across all three experimental conditions. All problem behavior during teaching trials was recorded as instances of problem behavior. The opportunity to view about a minute of a preferred video was used as reinforcement.
Results:
Results indicated that both learners engaged in higher rates of problem behavior during the slow teacher presentation. Both learners were presented with more instructional demands during the fast presentation. Both learners received more reinforcement during fast presentation than the medium and slow paced conditions. Results also indicated that both learners produced more responses during fast paced condition that the medium or slow paced conditions. Results also showed that there was no difference in percentage of correct responses for either learner during the three conditions.
Discussion:
Consistent with findings from previous studies, the results of this study illustrated that fast paced instruction produced positive outcomes on the frequency of problem behavior, magnitude of reinforcement, number of instructional demands and the number of responses for the participants in this study. However, the results of this study failed to illustrate that faster rates of instructional presentation increases correct responding. Despite this, the results promote the importance of fast paced instruction for children with autism.
A limitation of this study is that only a small number of participants were used; future research including a functional analysis of problem behavior prior to the implementation will add to an analysis of the differential effects of pace of instruction related to the functions of problem behavior.
Download summary of the study as PDF
Please note that every effort has been made to condense and provide a broad overview of this research. However in order not to lose the key information some of the information in this summary has been copied directly form the original article. All credits of the summary whether directly worded or re-worded are solely given to the researchers. Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in this paper on your child.
Please contact your ABA/VB consultant before implementing any of the procedures conducted in the paper on your child.
For the permission to post this study we thank: Dr. Vincent Carbone, Ed.D., BCBA
For the summary great thanks to: Miss Georgiana Elizabeth Barzey.
Responsible for the translation: Silke Johnson
o
17. Reducing pupils’ barriers to learning in a special needs school: integrating applied behavior analysis into Key Stages 1–3
Summary of the study
Laura Pitts, Stacey Gent and Marguerite L. Hoerger
Abstract:
Interventions based on applied behaviour analysis (ABA) have been shown to improve outcomes for young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), but there is very little research into outcomes for older pupils. This study evaluated the effectiveness of an ABA intervention for pupils with ASD and additional learning difficulties in a UK special needs school. The aim of the intervention was to reduce barriers to learning for individuals aged four to 13 years old. Behavior analysts collaborated with school-based teaching teams to design and implement function-based behavior support plans, individual education programs and ABA teaching strategies, and to promote the generalization of skills. Pupils were assessed at baseline and again following one academic year of intervention. The pupils demonstrated significant gains in learning skills, language and communication, social and play skills and self-help skills. Pupils of all ages acquired essential ‘learning to learn’ skills which have reduced their barriers to learning and are enabling them to learn more effectively. The article discusses how a behavioral model was successfully and affordably implemented across key stages within a special educational needs school.
Link to the full article: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/1467–8578.12251
o
18. Using applied behavior analysis as standard practice in a UK special needs school
Summary of the study
Denise Foran, Marguerite Hoerger, Hannah Philpott, Elin Walker Jones, J. Carl Hughes and Jonathan Morgan
Abstract:
This article describes how applied behavior analysis can be implemented effectively and affordably in a maintained special needs school in the UK. Behavior analysts collaborate with classroom teachers to provide early intensive behavior education for young children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and function based behavioral interventions for children between the ages of three and 18 years. Data are presented that show how the model is effective. Children with ASD under the age of seven made significant gains on intelligence quotient and on a range of skills including language, social and play, and academic skills following three academic terms of intervention. Case study data for two children reveal a marked decrease in challenging behavior following a function based behavioral intervention. These interventions have led to greater independence, integration and access to curricular activities. These data show that children are making significant gains within this cost-effective model.
Link to the full article: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/1467–8578.12088
o
19. Keep it to yourself? Parent emotion suppression influences physiological linkage and interaction behavior
Summary of the study
Sara F. Waters, Helena Rose Karnilowicz, Tessa V. West, Wendy Berry Mendes
Abstract:
Parents can influence children’s emotional responses through direct and subtle behavior. In this study we examined how parents’ acute stress responses might be transmitted to their 7- to 11-year-old children and how parental emotional suppression would affect parents’ and children’s physiological responses and behavior. Parents and their children (N = 214; Ndyads = 107; 47% fathers) completed a laboratory visit where we initially separated the parents and children and subjected the parent to a standardized laboratory stressor that reliably activates the body’s primary stress systems. Before reuniting with their children, parents were randomly assigned to either suppress their affective state—hide their emotions from their child—or to act naturally (control condition). Once reunited, parents and children completed a conflict conversation and two interaction tasks together. We measured their sympathetic nervous system (SNS) responses and observed interaction behavior. We obtained three key findings: (a) suppressing mothers’ SNS responses influenced their child’s SNS responses; (b) suppressing fathers’ SNS responses were influenced by their child’s SNS responses; and © dyads with suppressing parents appeared less warm and less engaged during interaction than control dyads. These findings reveal that parents’ emotion regulation efforts impact parent–child stress transmission and compromise interaction quality. Discussion focuses on short-term and long-term consequences of parental emotion regulation and children’s social-emotional development. (PsycInfo Database Record © 2020 APA, all rights reserved)
Reference and link to the article:
Waters, S. F., Karnilowicz, H. R., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2020). Keep it to yourself? Parent emotion suppression influences physiological linkage and interaction behavior. Journal of Family Psychology. Advance online publication. Link to the full article